Abstracts
OBJECTIVE: Lithium has been successfully employed to treat bipolar disorder for decades, and recently, was shown to attenuate the symptoms of other pathologies such as Alzheimer's disease, Down's syndrome, ischemic processes, and glutamate-mediated excitotoxicity. However, lithium's narrow therapeutic range limits its broader use. Therefore, the development of methods to better predict its dose becomes essential to an ideal therapy. METHOD: the performance of adult Wistar rats was evaluated at the open field and elevated plus maze after a six weeks treatment with chow supplemented with 0.255%, or 0.383% of lithium chloride, or normal feed. Thereafter, blood samples were collected to measure the serum lithium concentration. RESULTS: Animals fed with 0.255% lithium chloride supplemented chow presented a higher rearing frequency at the open field, and higher frequency of arms entrance at the elevated plus maze than animals fed with a 50% higher lithium dose presented. Nevertheless, both groups presented similar lithium plasmatic concentration. DISCUSSION: different behaviors induced by both lithium doses suggest that these animals had different lithium distribution in their brains that was not detected by lithium serum measurement. CONCLUSION: serum lithium concentration measurements do not seem to provide sufficient precision to support its use as predictive of behaviors.
Lithium chloride; Brain; Test; Open field test; Serum
OBJETIVO: Além de ser usado há décadas para tratar distúrbio bipolar, o lítio, mais recentemente, demonstrou-se eficaz para Alzheimer, síndrome de Down, processos isquêmicos e excitotoxicidade mediada por glutamato. Contudo, a estreita janela terapêutica do lítio limita seu uso. Portanto, o estabelecimento de métodos preditivos de dose torna-se importante. MÉTODO: O desempenho de ratos Wistar adultos foi avaliado no campo aberto e labirinto em cruz elevado após seis semanas de tratamento com uma ração suplementada com 0,255% ou 0,383% de cloreto de lítio ou ração normal. Coletou-se amostras de sangue para dosagem plasmática do lítio. RESULTADOS: Os animais alimentados com a ração com 0,255% de cloreto de lítio fizeram mais rearing no campo aberto e tiveram uma maior freqüência de entradas nos braços do labirinto elevado que os animais que ingeriram a dose mais alta. Apesar disso, verificou-se níveis plasmáticos de lítio semelhantes em ambos os grupos. DISCUSSÃO: A variação nos comportamentos destarte a presença de níveis plasmáticos semelhantes sugere que as diferentes doses produziram diferentes concentrações cerebrais não detectadas pela medida plasmática. CONCLUSÃO: Medidas da concentração plasmática de lítio não permitem prever de forma completa seus efeitos comportamentais.
Cloreto de lítio; Cérebro; Teste; Teste, campo aberto; Soro
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Staying at the crossroads: assessment of the potential of serum lithium monitoring in predicting an ideal lithium dose
Em uma encruzilhada: potencial do nível plasmático de lítio como preditor da dose ideal
Thiago Zaqueu Lima; Miriam Marcela Blanco; Jair Guilherme dos Santos Júnior; Carolina Tesone Coelho; Luiz Eugênio Mello
Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de São Paulo (Unifesp), São Paulo (SP), Brazil
Correspondence Correspondence Luiz Eugênio Mello Departmento de Fisiolgia Universidade Federal de São Paulo Rua Botucatu, 862 - 5 º andar 04023-062 São Paulo, SP, Brazil Phone: (+55 11) 5576-4513; Fax: (+55 11) 5579-2033 E-mail: lemello@unifesp.br
ABSTRACT
OBJECTIVE: Lithium has been successfully employed to treat bipolar disorder for decades, and recently, was shown to attenuate the symptoms of other pathologies such as Alzheimer's disease, Down's syndrome, ischemic processes, and glutamate-mediated excitotoxicity. However, lithium's narrow therapeutic range limits its broader use. Therefore, the development of methods to better predict its dose becomes essential to an ideal therapy.
METHOD: the performance of adult Wistar rats was evaluated at the open field and elevated plus maze after a six weeks treatment with chow supplemented with 0.255%, or 0.383% of lithium chloride, or normal feed. Thereafter, blood samples were collected to measure the serum lithium concentration.
RESULTS: Animals fed with 0.255% lithium chloride supplemented chow presented a higher rearing frequency at the open field, and higher frequency of arms entrance at the elevated plus maze than animals fed with a 50% higher lithium dose presented. Nevertheless, both groups presented similar lithium plasmatic concentration.
DISCUSSION: different behaviors induced by both lithium doses suggest that these animals had different lithium distribution in their brains that was not detected by lithium serum measurement.
CONCLUSION: serum lithium concentration measurements do not seem to provide sufficient precision to support its use as predictive of behaviors.
Descriptors: Lithium chloride; Brain, lithium levels; Test, anxiety; Open field test; Serum, lithium levels
RESUMO
OBJETIVO: Além de ser usado há décadas para tratar distúrbio bipolar, o lítio, mais recentemente, demonstrou-se eficaz para Alzheimer, síndrome de Down, processos isquêmicos e excitotoxicidade mediada por glutamato. Contudo, a estreita janela terapêutica do lítio limita seu uso. Portanto, o estabelecimento de métodos preditivos de dose torna-se importante.
MÉTODO: O desempenho de ratos Wistar adultos foi avaliado no campo aberto e labirinto em cruz elevado após seis semanas de tratamento com uma ração suplementada com 0,255% ou 0,383% de cloreto de lítio ou ração normal. Coletou-se amostras de sangue para dosagem plasmática do lítio.
RESULTADOS: Os animais alimentados com a ração com 0,255% de cloreto de lítio fizeram mais rearing no campo aberto e tiveram uma maior freqüência de entradas nos braços do labirinto elevado que os animais que ingeriram a dose mais alta. Apesar disso, verificou-se níveis plasmáticos de lítio semelhantes em ambos os grupos.
DISCUSSÃO: A variação nos comportamentos destarte a presença de níveis plasmáticos semelhantes sugere que as diferentes doses produziram diferentes concentrações cerebrais não detectadas pela medida plasmática.
CONCLUSÃO: Medidas da concentração plasmática de lítio não permitem prever de forma completa seus efeitos comportamentais.
Descritores: Cloreto de lítio; Cérebro, níveis de lítio; Teste, ansiedade; Teste, campo aberto; Soro, níveis de lítio
Introduction
Since its original proposal as a therapeutic means for the treatment of gout in 1817 and its more recent use in psychiatric patients in 1949 a long clinical experience has been developed with the use of lithium chloride. Yet, even for a compound used over such a long time there are still reports reminding of or describing for the first time aspects associated with its clinical use in neuropsychiatric patients.1,2 Recently, It was reported that children may be more susceptible to the consequences of overestimation of lithium dose, as lithium levels reach a steady-state balance faster in youths, leading to earlier toxicity.3 In addition, it was pointed that a delayed and slower equilibrium between lithium concentration at extracellular space and the inner compartment could result in delayed cardiac lithium poisoning.1 These recent discoveries highlight that there are a number of features on the clinical use of lithium that are yet to be revealed.
Lithium salts have been successfully used to treat especially the classical bipolar disorder.4-7 In spite of this, the narrow therapeutic range for the safe use of lithium induces physicians to avoid prescribing it, resulting in reduced consumption times of lithium salts. Conversely, as new studies demonstrate an increasing potential for the treatment of other pathologies the interest in using lithium salts was rekindled, being in vogue again. Recently, the use of lithium has been proven beneficial over animal models of Alzheimer's disease, Down's syndrome,8 lesions produced by ischemic processes and glutamate-mediated excitotoxicity,9,10 in patients affected by Canavan disease,11 and in subjects with a high risk to develop psychosis.12 Moreover, lithium seems to protect neurons13 and stimulate the proliferation and migration of neural stems cells.12,14,15 This current expanding use of lithium resumes a need for methods able to monitor the lithium levels, in order to adjust its dosage to reach therapeutic effects, besides avoiding its toxicity.
Lithium's therapeutic window for human clinical use is between: 0.6 to 1.2 mMol/L serum levels. Thus lithium plasmatic levels below the minimum limit result in a higher than acceptable relapse risk of manic-depressive symptoms, while serum levels above the maximal limit produce intoxication.16,17 Although the security range has been established, this does not exclude the occasional toxic consequences of lithium. Neurological features may be encountered early, including drowsiness, slurred speech, psychomotor slowing, and impaired memory and, in severe cases, seizures, coma and death.18 Given that lithium is exclusively eliminated by kidneys, its therapy is associated with a progressive decline in creatinine clearance and an estimated interval of 20 years until end-stage renal failure.19,20
The toxicity of lithium is related to its distribution, which is determined, mainly, by passive diffusion modulated by bicarbonate. Lithium can be also transported by ionic pumps with ATP consumption, and through channels coupled to the Na+ driving force that in turn is maintained by the associated Na+/K+ pump.21 The active transport of lithium allows its concentration at specific systems and organs of the body, such as central nervous system (CNS) and muscles.22 Actually, lithium levels vary even in the CNS, where there is a higher concentration in the brain than in the spinal cord,23 and the lithium distribution through brain structures may also vary according to the administered doses.24 In this way, measurements of lithium in a system may not necessarily predict its levels in another one. Therefore, this work aimed at evaluating the accuracy and relevance of the plasmatic lithium measurement aiming to estimate CNS levels in well-controlled laboratory conditions.
Method
Three-month-old Wistar rats, of both genders, were housed under standard laboratory conditions with light/dark cycle (12/12h), lights on at 7:00 AM, and food and water were provided ad libitum. All experimental protocols were approved by the Animal Care and Use Ethics Committee of Universidade Federal de São Paulo. Animals were distributed to experimental groups according to the different treatments over a period of six weeks. Thus, animals fed with conventional rat chow (Nuvilab, Brazil) constituted the control group (n = 10); animals that received a 0.255% rich lithium chloride (LiCl) rat chow constituted the 0.255 group (n = 8); and finally, group 0.383 was composed by 8 animals fed with a 0.383% LiCl rich feed. The 0.255% lithium supplemented feed has already been demonstrated to result in a plasmatic level within the therapeutic ranges.25 Here we chose to compare the plasmatic levels and the behavioral effect of this dose with a 50% higher dose (0.383%). The body weight of all animals was evaluated over the six-week period. At the end of the treatment, the animals were tested at an open field and elevated plus maze task. And finally, blood samples were collected through a small incision on the tail vein, for measuring lithium plasmatic concentration.
1. Treatment
The lithium rich feed was produced by mixing conventional commercial chow dust with a watery lithium chloride solution of known concentration. After that the rat chow pellet was extruded and dried in an oven.
2. Open field task
After a habituation period, the exploratory behavior of all animals was analyzed at an 87 cm diameter wooden open field surrounded by a 20 cm high polyvinyl chloride wall. The apparatus's floor was divided in twelve compartments by lines painted at the surface of a wooden disk.
In the beginning of the task, the animal was positioned in the center of the apparatus, and was allowed to freely explore it for three minutes. The number of times the animals crossed a line, i.e. compartments crossed, was measured as a quantification of horizontal locomotor activity. The vertical locomotor activity was assessed by measurement of the rearing frequency. The time spent in freezing and grooming behavior was also recorded (as an indication of fear and anxiety emotional states).
3. Elevated plus maze
The maze apparatus was comprised by two arms surrounded by 30 cm high wooden walls that cross with two open arms, resulting in a central compartment. Both arms were 49 cm in length and 10cm in width and were placed 50 cm above the floor.
After a five-minute task, the elevated plus maze allows to investigate the anxiety emotional state of rats through the quantification of the time spent in each arm. This apparatus also allows the acquisition of data from horizontal locomotor activity by means of quantifying the frequency of entrance in each compartment.
4. Plasmatic lithium level measurement
Finally, blood samples of all animals were collected through a small incision on tail vein, after ether anesthesia. Then, the plasmatic lithium levels of these blood samples were measured by the Crown-Ether method.
5. Statistical analyses
Data, that fit into a normal distribution, as feed ingestion and body weight, were analyzed by one-factor repeated measure ANOVA. Data that did not fit a normal distribution (Open field, elevated plus maze and plasmatic lithium level) were analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis, followed the post-hoc test of Multiple Comparison of Mean Ranks MCMR. For all statistical procedures, it was adopted a significance level (α) of 0.05.
Results
1. Feed consumption and body weight progression
Groups treated with both doses of LiCl-rich feed presented a reduced chow consumption as compared to the control group (p = 0.03). However, in spite of the food ingestion attenuation caused by lithium intake, no difference was found between the higher (group 0.383) and lower doses (group 0.255), as indicated in Figure 1.
Both groups treated with lithium rich chow showed a diminished body weight gain as compared with control animals (repeated measures ANOVA, p < 0.0001 followed by Tukey-Kramer, p < 0.05). As shown in Figure 2, lithium-treated animals gained mass more slowly than control rats, from the third treatment week onwards, resulting in a lower body weight at the end of lithium administration.
2. Open field
Animals fed with higher lithium dose chow (group 0.383) showed a smaller (horizontal) locomotor activity through central compartments of the open field as compared to the control group (Figure 3B) (p = 0.024, Kruskal-Wallis followed by MCMR post-hoc test). In spite of a 2.4 times smaller locomotion presented by group 0.383 relative to group 0.255, this did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.0587). In addition, group 0.383 showed a significant 45% smaller rearing frequency than the control group did and a significant 55% reduction as compared to the 0.255 group (Figure 3C). Moreover, group 0.383 presented a 5.2 and 3 times longer mean freezing duration than the control and 0.255 groups did, respectively (Figure 3D), but this, however, only achieved significance between group 0.383 and controls (p = 0.044, Kruskal-Wallis followed by MCMR post-hoc test). In contrast, the comparison between animals that received the smaller dose of LiCl and control animals produced only a trend (p = 0.079) in increasing the duration of freezing.
3. Elevated plus maze
While the 0.383 group presented the same total entrance (open plus closed arms) frequency as control animals, the 0.255 group, in spite of having received a smaller dose of lithium, showed a greater absolute number of entries than did both other groups (p = 0.008, Kruskal-Wallis followed by MCMR post-hoc test), as illustrated by Figure 4A. Moreover, animals fed with the 0.255% lithium-rich diet demonstrated a reduced relative entrance frequency in the open arms (Figure 4B), as compared to the control and 0.383 groups (p = 0.03, Kruskal-Wallis followed by MCMR post-hoc test). Nevertheless, such a difference should be carefully analyzed, given that there was no difference among the relative time spent in the open arms by any experimental group (Figure 4C - Kruskal-Wallis, p > 0.05). Therefore, the effects induced by the treatment with the smaller dose of lithium chloride should be considered as a disturbance on animal's behavior (thus not similar to the human mood disorder), rather than be classified as a parallel to human anxiety.
4. Plasmatic lithium level measurement
At the end of the six-week treatment, the plasmatic lithium level of the groups treated with both doses of LiCl was significantly higher than that of control animals (p = 0.013, Kruskal-Wallis followed by MCMR post-hoc test). However, in spite of having received lithium doses with 50% difference, groups 0.255 and 0.383 yielded the same lithium serum concentration (Figure 5, p = 0.73).
Discussion
1. Feed consumption and body weight progression
Animals fed with both doses of LiCl showed reduced food ingestion, what confirms data already collected,26 but the chow consumption by both lithium-treated groups was always the same, excluding the possibility that the same lithium serum levels could result from distinct amount of drug ingestion. Given that lithium intake can result in visceral illness,27-29 it was expected that lithium-treated animals would present an attenuated body weight gain, as it is indeed confirmed in Figure 2.
2. Open field task
A central zone locomotion difference was encountered between control and 0.383 groups. Given that, the total locomotion of all groups was equal (Figure 3B, p = 0.2346), and such a divergence should be considered as a signal of avoidance during the exploration of the apparatus, rather than altered horizontal locomotion. The lack of divergence in total locomotion among the groups presented here is in disagreement with already published studies,27,29,30 which demonstrated a suppressive effect of lithium salts over the horizontal locomotor activity. In the work of Smith,27 rats developed a lithium plasmatic level similar to that shown here (0.66 mMol/L ± 0.05), although they clearly showed a smaller locomotion after treatment with 1.5 mMol/L lithium for four days. It is likely that the nature of the lithium treatment employed here resulted in a heterogeneous lithium distribution through brain structures. Therefore, the animals evaluated in the present study showed more variability in open field variables analyzed, masking a possible effect of lithium on the evaluated parameters. While there are studies in which rats had received a precise amount of lithium salts by gavage,27 or by intraperitoneal administration,30 here, lithium was supplied in supplemented chow. This chow was freely available for the rats to ingest. Obviously the consumption rate can thus be more variable. Moreover, the difference in the duration of the lithium treatment could explain why the treatment employed here failed in corroborating the suppressive effect of lithium salts over horizontal locomotor activity, as demonstrated in the above mentioned studies. Such papers shown a suppressive effect of lithium upon acute drug administration,30 or have sustained the lithium treatment for a shorter time27 than performed here, suggesting a possible habituation to lithium's effects during a long consumption time.
Despite the lack of a suppressive effect of lithium treatment over the horizontal locomotion, the group fed with the higher lithium dose explored the central compartment of the apparatus less often than the control group did. Moreover, the high-dose lithium-treated group also showed longer freezing time in this test than control animals. These behavioral consequences could be seen as suggestive of an anxiogenic effect of this high-dose treatment. Yet this finding was not confirmed by the more specific test for the assessment of anxiety (the elevated plus maze). Therefore, the smaller time spent in the central compartment of the open field test by the lithium-treated group under the higher dose does not unequivocally equate with greater anxiety, and might be more conservatively rated as an avoidance state
The higher dose of lithium reduced the vertical locomotor activity, as shown by a diminished rearing frequency of the 0.383 group. This suppressive action of lithium strengthens the data on the already known effect of lithium.27,30,31 Interestingly, the doses of lithium did not produce the same behavioral effect in both groups of lithium-fed animals, in spite of the similar changes observed in the lithium serum levels of both groups. Therefore, lithium serum measurement did not allow the prediction of its levels in the brain.
3. Elevated plus maze task
The smaller open/total arms entrance rate of the 0.255 group as compared to control animals suggests a preference of the lithium-treated rats for the closed and thus, less exposed arms. This sign of anxiety was not confirmed as such given the lack of an increase of time spent in the closed arms (the "safer" compartment). Despite the well known mood stabilizing properties of lithium over emotional disorders, its anxiolytic or anxiogenic effects over emotional state of normal animals are less explored. Wood and colleagues, in agreement with our current findings, did not find an anxiogenic consequence of the treatment with lithium carbonate supplemented chow for 35 days in adult rats.32 On the other hand, Youngs and colleagues argued that adolescent rats (45-day old, rather than 3 months of age as employed in the present study) maintained an anxiogenic effect of lithium for some weeks after the end of treatment,33 in spite of having been fed with lithium carbonate supplemented chow for a shorter time than here. Regardless of the above discrepancies or similarities with our current findings, we should remark that here we observed a dissimilar outcome for the two lithium-treated groups in the elevated plus maze despite similar plasmatic lithium levels. Animals that received the lower lithium dose (the 0.255 group) presented a greater number of entries in both open and closed arms as compared to others groups. Such a difference might be an indicative of a greater horizontal activity produced by the lower lithium dose in this test rather than an indication of anxiety. Nevertheless, this enhancement in horizontal locomotion must be carefully interpreted, because the same effect was not observed in the open field task. Moreover, this effect is in disagreement with already-published reports, which found no change in the elevated plus maze as a consequence of the treatment of Sprague-Dawley rats with 0.24% lithium carbonate supplemented chow for 35 days.32 Therefore here again, lithium plasmatic measurements did not seem to clearly indicate what would be the lithium levels in specific brain structures related to the analyzed behavior.
4. Plasmatic lithium concentration
Lithium is not a perfect drug due to the occurrence of short- and long-term side effects. However, it is the most effective drug to treat classical bipolar disorder, whose consequences may be even worse than lithium's toxicity. Without some form of therapy, up to 20% of bipolar disorder patients will commit suicide,34 and lithium treatment reduces this rate by a factor of eight. Therefore, lithium therapeutic effects compensate its toxic effects making it an alternative that should be better understood rather than so easily discarded.
In spite of new methods that have been developed to better estimate a precise dose of lithium,2,3,5,17,35-39 many of these technologies are based in the serum level measurement. However, lithium does not have a uniform distribution through body compartments. In fact, its concentration in the brain, where it exerts its therapeutic actions, is higher than in the blood.6,7,24 It is very difficult to estimate CNS lithium levels, as serum and red blood cell concentration does not closely correlate with brain levels, with the coefficient for the strongest correlation (between serum and brain) remaining at 0.66.
Some authors have already pointed that lithium serum concentration did not satisfactorily reflect the tissue level, and therefore, could not predict the occurrence of toxicity.1,40 Here, some behavioral effects of two doses of lithium (after a six-week treatment) were measured at the same time that plasmatic lithium concentrations were obtained. According to the data presented above, both lithium doses resulted in similar plasmatic levels, although rat chows had different lithium concentrations and similar food consumption levels. Nevertheless, these two groups with equal lithium plasmatic levels, showed significantly different behavioral responses in some of the behavioral tests evaluated here. This suggests that the different lithium concentration of the ingested food resulted in different brain concentrations and/or distributions, and, although undetectable by serum monitoring, may lead to distinct behavioral responses.
The experimental evidence presented here, in well-controlled laboratory conditions, corroborate the notion that lithium serum measurement does not provide an estimative of brain lithium level that is precise enough to predict its effects over the CNS.
Conclusion
The measurement of serum lithium concentration, which is the diagnostic method currently used to monitor lithium levels in bipolar patients, did not detect changes in brain lithium levels, despite an increase of 50% in the lithium dose ingested. Behavioral tests, however, pointed to different cognitive performances, suggesting that the groups had different lithium brain concentrations or distributions that were sufficient to change their cognitive state. Therefore, serum monitoring seems to be inaccurate to ensure the safe use of lithium. New methods to predict the best lithium dosages for patients are required to allow the growing clinical use of lithium salts.
Acknowledgments
This work would not have been possible without the technical assistance provided by Mauro Cardoso Pereira and Ivone de Paulo.
Disclosures
Submited: January 22, 2008
Accepted: June 25, 2008
References
- 1. Waring WS. Delayed cardiotoxicity in chronic lithium poisoning: discrepancy between serum lithium concentrations and clinical status. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 2007;100(5):353-5.
- 2. Chiu CC, Shen WW, Chen KP, Lu ML. Application of the Cockcroft-Gault method to estimate lithium dosage requirement. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;61(3):269-74.
- 3. Hagino OR, Weller EB, Weller RA, Fristad MA. Comparison of lithium dosage methods for preschool- and early school-age children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998;37(1):60-5.
- 4. Thase ME. Bipolar depression: diagnostic and treatment considerations. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18(4):1213-30.
- 5. Kato T, Inubushi T, Takahashi S. Relationship of lithium concentrations in the brain measured by lithium-7 magnetic resonance spectroscopy to treatment response in mania. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1994;14(5):330-5.
- 6. Ward ME, Musa MN, Bailey L. Clinical pharmacokinetics of lithium. J Clin Pharmacol 1994;34(4):280-5.
- 7. Wang PW, Ketter TA. Pharmacokinetics of mood stabilizers and new anticonvulsants. Psychopharmacol Bull 2002;36(1):44-66.
- 8. Phiel CJ, Wilson CA, Lee VM, Klein PS. GSK-3alpha regulates production of Alzheimer's disease amyloid-beta peptides. Nature 2003;423(6938):435-9.
- 9. Ren M, Senatorov VV, Chen RW, Chuang DM. Postinsult treatment with lithium reduces brain damage and facilitates neurological recovery in a rat ischemia/reperfusion model. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 2003;100(10):6210-5.
- 10. Wada A, Yokoo H, Yanagita T, Kobayashi H. Lithium: potential therapeutics against acute brain injuries and chronic neurodegenerative diseases. J Pharmacol Sci 2005;99(4):307-21.
- 11. Janson CG, Assadi M, Francis J, Bilaniuk L, Shera D, Leone P. Lithium citrate for Canavan disease. Pediatr Neurol 2005;33(4):235-43.
- 12. Pilcher HR. Drug research: the ups and downs of lithium. Nature 2003;425(6954):118-20.
- 13. Moore GJ, Bebchuk JM, Wilds IB, Chen G, Manji HK. Lithium-induced increase in human brain grey matter. Lancet. 2000;356(9237):1241-2. Erratum in: Lancet 2000;356(9247):2104.
- 14. Hashimoto R, Senatorov V, Kanai H, Leeds P, Chuang DM. Lithium stimulates progenitor proliferation in cultured brain neurons. Neuroscience 2003;117(1):55-61.
- 15. Kim JS, Chang MY, Yu IT, Kim JH, Lee SH, Lee YS, Son H. Lithium selectively increases neuronal differentiation of hippocampus neural progenitor cells both in vitro and in vivo. J Neurochem 2004;89(2):324-36.
- 16. Linder MW, Keck PE Jr. Standards of laboratory practice: antidepressant drug monitoring. National Academy of Clinical Biochemistry. Clin Chem 1998;44(5):1073-84.
- 17. Amdisen A. Serum concentration and clinical supervision in monitoring of lithium treatment. Ther Drug Monit 1980;2(1):73-83.
- 18. Bartha L, Marksteiner J, Bauer G, Benke T. Persistent cognitive deficits associated with lithium intoxication: a neuropsychological case description. Cortex 2002;38(5):743-52.
- 19. Presne C, Fakhouri F, Noel LH, Stengel B, Even C, Kreis H, Mignon F, Grünfeld JP. Lithium-induced nephropathy: rate of progression and prognostic factors. Kidney Int 2003;64(2):585-92.
- 20. Livingstone C, Rampes H. Lithium: a review of its metabolic adverse effects. J Psychopharmacol 2006;20(3):347-55.
- 21. Frazer A, Mendels J, Brunswick D, London J, Pring M, Ramsey TA, Rybakowski J. Erythrocyte concentrations of the lithium ion: clinical correlates and mechanisms of action. Am J Psychiatry 1978;135(9):1065-9.
- 22. Finley PR, Warner MD, Peabody CA. Clinical relevance of drug interactions with lithium. Clin Pharmacokinet. 1995;29(3):172-91.
- 23. Levine S, Saltzman A, Katof B, Meister A, Cooper TB. Lithium distribution in experimental inflammation of brain and spinal cord. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1996;20(6):1011-7.
- 24. Ramaprasad S. Lithium spectroscopic imaging of rat brain at therapeutic doses. Mag Resonance Imaging 2004;22(5):727-34.
- 25. Gould TD, Chen G, Manji HK. In vivo evidence in the brain for lithium inhibition of glycogen synthase kinase-3. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;29(1):32-8.
- 26. Curtis KS, Sved AF, Verbalis JG, Stricker EM. Lithium chloride-induced anorexia, but not conditioned taste aversions, in rats with area postrema lesions. Brain Res 1994;663(1):30-7.
- 27. Smith DF. Lithium and carbamazepine: effects on learned taste aversion and open field behavior in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1983;18(4):483-8.
- 28. Ossenkopp KP, Eckel LA. Toxin-induced conditioned changes in taste reactivity and the role of the chemosensitive area postrema. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995;19(1):99-108.
- 29. Parker LA. Taste avoidance and taste aversion: evidence for two different processes. Learn Behav 2003;31(2):165-72.
- 30. Tenk CM, Kavaliers M, Ossenkopp KP. Dose response effects of lithium chloride on conditioned place aversions and locomotor activity in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 2005;515(1-3):117-27.
- 31. Johnson FN. Lithium effects upon components of activity in rats. Experientia 1976;32(2):212-4.
- 32. Wood GE, Young LT, Reagan LP, Chen B, McEwen BS. Stress-induced structural remodeling in hippocampus: prevention by lithium treatment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004;101(11):3973-8.
- 33. Youngs RM, Chu MS, Meloni EG, Naydenov A, Carlezon WA Jr., Konradi C. Lithium administration to preadolescent rats causes long-lasting increases in anxiety-like behavior and has molecular consequences. J Neurosci 2006;26(22):6031-9.
- 34. Tondo L, Hennen J, Baldessarini RJ. Lower suicide risk with long-term lithium treatment in major affective illness: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001;104(3):163-72.
- 35. Alda M. Method for prediction of serum lithium levels. Biol Psychiatry 1988;24(2):218-24.
- 36. Gervasoni N, Zona-Favre MP, Osiek C, Roth L, Bondolfi G, Bertschy G. Lithium dose prediction based on 24 hours single dose levels: a prospective evaluation. Pharmacol Res 2003;48(6):649-53.
- 37. Marr MA, Djuric PE, Ritschel WA, Garver DL. Prediction of lithium carbonate dosage in psychiatric inpatients using the repeated one-point method. Clin Pharm 1983;2(3):243-8.
- 38. Naganuma H, Akiyoshi J, Fujii I. Prediction of optimal lithium dosage. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1988;8(1):66-7.
- 39. Srisurapanont M, Pratoomsri W, Maneeton N. Evaluation of three simple methods for predicting therapeutic lithium doses. Psychiatry Res. 2000;94(1):83-8.
- 40. Kato T, Fujii K, Shioiri T, Inubushi T, Takahashi S. Lithium side effects in relation to brain lithium concentration measured by lithium-7 magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1996;20(1):87-97.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
25 Sept 2008 -
Date of issue
Sept 2008
History
-
Received
22 Jan 2008 -
Accepted
25 June 2008