Abstract
Objective
This study investigated the effects of WN on LE in subjects with chronic tinnitus and normal hearing thresholds. The study was a prospective, non-randomized, before-and-after, intra-participant intervention.
Methods
Twenty-five subjects performed the following tests: conventional and high-frequency audiometry, acuphenometry, screening questionnaires for depression and anxiety symptoms, Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (THI), Montreal Cognitive Assessment, and high WM test from the Working Memory Assessment Battery, Federal University of Minas Gerais (WMAB) as the LE measure in two conditions: No Added Noise (NAN) and with Added Noise (AN).
Results
Seventeen participants (68%) performed better on AN condition. Data analysis revealed a 45% improvement in the WMAB total span count on AN setting, with a significant p value (p = 0.001).
Conclusion
The subgroup of participants without traces of anxiety symptoms, up to mild traces of depressive symptoms, having unilateral tinnitus, and a THI level up to grade 2, had improved WM performance in the presence of WN, which suggests a release of cognitive resources and less auditory effort under these combined conditions.
Evidence level
4.
Keywords
Tinnitus; Hearing; Auditory perception; Memory; Noise
Highlights
White noise improved high working memory in 68% of tinnitus patients.
White noise reduced high working memory in 8% of tinnitus patients.
White noise had no effect in high working memory in 24% of tinnitus patients.
White noise could release of cognitive resources and reduce auditory effort.
Introduction
The prevalence of speech misunderstandings among patients with tinnitus and normal hearing is 37.5%.11 Tyler RS, Baker LJ. Difficulties experienced by tinnitus sufferers. J Speech Hearing Disord. 1983;48:150-4. These patients report speech comprehension impairment and direct interference of tinnitus in their communication ability.22 Goldstein B, Shulman A. Central auditory speech test findings in individuals with subjective idiopathic tinnitus. Int Tinnitus J. 1999;5:16-9. Listening Effort (LE) is defined as “the deliberate allocation of mental resources to overcome obstacles in goal pursuit when carrying out a task, when tasks involve listening”.33 Pichora-Fuller MK, Kramer SE, Eckert MA, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear Hear. 2016;37(Suppl 1):S5-S27. There is an inverse correlation between Working Memory (WM) and LE; the greater the cognitive skill, the lower the LE.44 Rönnberg J, Lunner T, Zekveld A, et al. The Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model: theoretical, empirical, and clinical advances. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013;7:31. Span tests have been used to evaluate high WM (Cattel-Horn-Carroll Theory and Miyake taxonomy), the main trademarks of which are the storage, handling, and processing of several information flows for a coordinated response.55 Webb SL, Loh V, Lampit A, Bateman JE, Birney DP. Meta-analysis of the effects of computerized cognitive training on executive functions: A cross-disciplinary taxonomy for classifying outcome cognitive factors. Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28:232-50. The effect of tinnitus on auditory perception is still poorly understood, contributing to the lack of effective management of this complaint. There is still no consensus about the influence of tinnitus on WM perhaps due methodology variability. Some research reports a negative influence of tinnitus (increase cognitive load and decrease residual ability on task performance)66 Rossiter S, Stevens C, Walker G. Tinnitus and its effect on working memory and attention. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49:150-60.
7 Degeest S, Keppler H, Corthals P. The effect of tinnitus on listening effort in normal-hearing young adults: a preliminary study. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60:1036-45.
8 Gudwani S, Munjal SK, Panda NK, Kohli A. Association of chronic subjective tinnitus with neuro- cognitive performance. Int Tinnitus J. 2017;21:90-7.
9 Tavanai E, Mohammadkhani G. A different view on the link between tinnitus and cognition; is there a reciprocal link? Int J Neurosci. 2018;128:1188-98.
10 Wang Y, Zhang J-N, Hu W, Li J-J, 6 Zhou J-X, Zhang J-P, et al. The characteristics of cognitive impairment in subjective chronic tinnitus. Brain Behav. 2018(3):e00918.
11 Nagaraj MK, Bhaskar A, Prabhu P. Assessment of auditory working memory in normal hearing adults with tinnitus. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2020;277:47-54.-1212 Sharma A, Mohanty M, Panda N, Munjal S. Impact of hearing loss on cognitive abilities in subjects with tinnitus. Neurol India. 2022;70:554-62. and others claims no influence at all.1313 Mohamad N, Hoare DJ, Hall DA. The consequences of tinnitus and tinnitus severity on cognition: A review of the behavioural evidence. Hear Res. 2015;332:199-209.
14 Waechter S, Hallendorf L, Malmstein E, Olsson A, Brännström KJ. The impact of tinnitus on n-back performance in normal hearing individuals. J Am Acad Audiol. 2019;30:169-77-1515 Waechter S, Wilson, WJ, Brännström JK. The impact of tinnitus on working memory capacity. Int J Audiol. 2021;60:274-81. Although, a structured systematic review indicated poor evidence that white noise decreases tinnitus loudness or perceived tinnitus severity,1616 Hobson J, Chisholm E, Refaie AE. Sound therapy (masking) in the management of tinnitus in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010;(12):CD006371. in clinical practice, the use of a sound generator with White Noise (WN) may help treat tinnitus annoyance1717 Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Tinnitus retraining therapy: a different view on tinnitus. ORL J Otorhinolaryngol Relat Spec. 2006;68:23-9.,1818 Sushun Z, Tyler R. A review of sound therapies for tinnitus. Ann Otolaryngol Rhinol. 2020;7:1233. through its perception decay. Our suspicion that tinnitus crowds WM11 and that WN is helpful in tinnitus sound therapy prompted this study. Our hypothesis was that the presence of WN during a high WM task in participants with chronic tinnitus and normal hearing thresholds would improve cognitive performance and reduce LE. The objective of this study was to compare the LE of these individuals in two diverse settings: No Added Noise (NAN) and Added Noise (AN).
Methods
This prospective, non-randomized, intra-participant, before-and-after intervention study was carried out from 2020 to 2022 at Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade São Paulo (FMUSP-HC) and private clinic. The design was based in pilot study results, carried out with five control and four tinnitus subjects. In noise, all control subjects performed worse or equal and all tinnitus subjects performed better compared to the silent condition. All study participants provided informed consent, and the study design was approved by the appropriate ethics review board (ethical approval CAAE 89320018.6.0000.005, version 2). Strict non probabilistic sampling was used in this study. Participants were recruited from the general audiology clinic (hospital) and private office. For sample size calculation, we considered a minimum clinically significant effect of one test unit, global mean of 4.82 and standard deviation of 1.52 (data from Brazilian validation1919 Wood GMO, Carvalho MRS, Rothe-Neves R, Haase VG. Validation of the working memory assessment battery at UFMG (Validação da bateria de avaliação da memória de trabalho na UFMG). Psicol Refl Crít. 2001;14:325-41.) and the PS Size Calculation program 3.0 was used. A sample of 23 participants was estimated in repeated observations in both settings to reach a statistical power of 85% and a 2-tailed significance level of 5%.
The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) Brazilian population fluent in Brazilian Portuguese; (2) At least 4-years of education; (3) Continuous uni-or bilateral tinnitus for more than 6-months to ensure tinnitus presence during all procedure; (4) Normal hearing thresholds (0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz up to 20 dB HL)2020 World Health Organization. World report on hearing. March 3, 2021. Available at: https://www.who.int/teams/noncommunicable-diseases/sensory-functions-disability-and-rehabilitation/highlighting-priorities-for-ear-and-hearing-care. Accessed June 29, 2023.
https://www.who.int/teams/noncommunicabl...
; and (5) A minimum Speech Recognition Index of 96%. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) Use of any drug for tinnitus treatment, or which may impair cognition; (2) Anxiety or depression; (3) Learning disorders; (4) Hyperacusis; and (5) Pulsatile, rhythmic tinnitus, and/or myoclonus.
Assessments included anamnesis (years of education, tinnitus onset, type, side and worst side), otoscopy, hearing tests, LE measurements (WM test), cognitive evaluation, and questionnaires. The hearing test, performed in a soundproof booth, included (1) Conventional warble tone audiometry (0.25-8 kHz), (2) The most comfortable level for speech (MCL-performed with headphones, the subject was instructed to inform the most comfortable voice level), (3) High-Frequency Audiometry (HFA) (9-20 kHz) with warble tone, (4) Tinnitus pitch and loudness,2121 Henry JA, Zaugg TL, Schechter MA. Clinical guide for audiologic tinnitus management I: assessment. Am J Audiol. 2005;14:21-48. and (5) MCLwn (performed with headphones, the subject was instructed to inform the most comfortable WN level). Audiometers and headphones R37a/DD45 (Resonance) and AC40/HDA300 (Interacoustics) were used. The Working Memory Assessment Battery, Federal University of Minas Gerais (WMAB),1919 Wood GMO, Carvalho MRS, Rothe-Neves R, Haase VG. Validation of the working memory assessment battery at UFMG (Validação da bateria de avaliação da memória de trabalho na UFMG). Psicol Refl Crít. 2001;14:325-41. the only validated instrument for the Brazilian Portuguese language was used to assess high WM. It was developed in accordance with the previous work of Salthouse and Babcock.2222 Salthouse TA, Babcock RL. Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:763-76. The test evaluated the effectiveness of information storage and processing (listening span). A verbal response was chosen to avoid the influence of writing disorders. The test was performed with headphones in both ears simultaneously, with a previously recorded stimulus (at MCL), in two different settings: (1) NAN and (2) AN (monotically due to audiometer limitation, on the side with the worst tinnitus, under MCLwn). The rest interval between them was 5 min. Different sentences were used in each setting to avoid memorization. The test condition order was randomized in two blocks (NAN/AN and AN/NAN) to prevent learning influence. The participants were previously trained. On each stage the subject hears a set of sentences, and two answers are expected. Example: “The mechanic changed the tire and wheel./Who?” (sentence/question); “the mechanic” (answer 1) and “wheel” (answer 2/span). The test complexity increases with the greater number of spans to be remembered (Fig. 1). The test had ten stages, and each stage had three different sentence sets (three attempts). It was completed when the participant was unable to correctly answer two out of three attempts. The outcome measure was the total span count result. We considered the increase of correct spans a decrease of LE. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) has also been applied to cognitive evaluation.2323 Sarmento ALR. Presentation and applicability of the Brazilian version of the MoCA for screening mild cognitive impairment. Master dissertation. Universidade Federal de São Paulo - Escola Paulista de Medicina; 2009. Accessed June 29, 20https://repositorio.unifesp.br/bitstream/handle/11600/8967/Publico-00377.pdf.
https://repositorio.unifesp.br/bitstream...
The Patient Health Questionnaire-9/PHQ-92424 Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-9 - validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Int Med. 2001;16:606-13. and Generalized Anxiety Disorder Assessment/GAD-72525 Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, Lowe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Intern Med. 2006;16:1092-7. were administered for depression and anxiety symptoms screening, and Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (THI)2626 Ferreira PEA, Cunha F, Onishi ET, Branco-Barreiro FCA, Ganança FF. Tinnitus handicap inventory: cultural modifications for Brazilian Portuguese (Tinnitus handicap inventory: adaptação cultural para o Português Brasileiro). Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2005;17:303-10. for tinnitus distress. To compare the WMAB NAN and AN setting result (negative binomial distribution data ‒ Quasi Likelihood under Independence Model Criterion/QIC value of 42.833), the Wilcoxon signed rank test, the Generalized Estimating Equation (GEE) model with a link function based on the negative binomial distribution with an exchangeable correlation matrix were used. The magnitude of the effect was expressed by the ratio of the means with their respective 95% Confidence Interval; p-values below 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Data was additionally modeled for potential confounding effects. The SPSS program (version 25.0; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) was used for the analyses.
Results
The sample consisted of 25 individuals (68% women and 32% men); their characteristics are presented in Table 1. The mean duration of tinnitus was 8.5-years and the high pitch was more prevalent.
Audiological evaluation was performed after otoscopy. The 50 ears analysis (average threshold) revealed: 9.55 ± 4.81 dB HL (0.25-2 kHz) and 10.06 ± 6.63 dB HL (3-8 kHz). In HFA, greater variability was observed: 20.3 ± 4.81 dB HL (9-12.5 kHz) and 23 ± 22.8 dB HL (14-20 kHz).
Regarding the effect of noise on LE (total span count of the WMAB), 17 participants performed better in the noisy setting (Table 2). All applied statistics tests were significant: p = 0.001 for Wilcoxon signed rank test and p = 0.000 for Wald Chi Square in GEE (Table 3). The mean ratio inversion (1 divided by mean ratio 0.686) calculation showed a performance improvement of 45% on AN condition. However, there was an overlap in Confidence Intervals (IC) of NAN and AN outcome (Fig. 2); therefore, there was no statistical difference between the two conditions, which prevented us from extrapolating these data to similar tinnitus population. Thus, the presence of WN improved WM performance in a subgroup of 17 participants (68%) with: (a) Up to mild traces of depression symptoms (88%), (b) No traces of anxiety symptoms (76%), (c) THI grades 1 and 2, (d) Tinnitus for more than two years, (e) Loudness of up to 15 dB (53%) and (f) Unilateral tinnitus (70%). The preferred WN loudness was 30-35 Db (58%). Approximately 60% of the sample required a minimal Signal-to-Noise ratio (S/N) of +20 dB.
WMAB confidence intervals. WMAB, Working Memory Assessment Battery; CI, Confidence Interval; NAN, No Added Noise; AN, Added Noise.
Description of mean, median, standard deviation, statistical tests and mean ratio of WMAB total span count in NAN and AN settings.
We explored the effect of sex, age, MoCA, tinnitus onset and type, years of education, hearing thresholds, MCL, MCLwn, S/N ratio, THI, PHQ-9 and GAD-7 on the magnitude variation of WN effect on the WMAB total span count. We did not find any significant findings except for: age > 50 years mean ratio 0.003 (95% CI 0.974-0.995), years of education mean ratio 0.000 (95% CI 1.056-1.118), cognitive test performance (MoCA) mean ratio 0.003 (95% CI 1.395-4.845), and THI mean ratio 0.008 (95% CI 0.975-0.996). As age increased, the performance discrepancy between settings decreased and the influence of WN on WM became less evident. Years of education and cognition impacted the mean; that is, the better the MoCA test results and/or education level, the greater the WMAB test performance in both settings (NAN and AN). Regarding THI, the higher the score, the worse the WM performance.
Discussion
Seventeen participants (68%) performed better in the WN condition, 6 (24%) showed the same performance, and only 2 (8%) performed worst with noise. These results suggest that WN improves WM by some means although there was no statistical difference between the NAN and AN condition on CI.
This finding may be explained by the physical phenomenon, the Stochastic Resonance (SR). In neuroscience, the term SR has been used to describe a phenomenon in which a very weak signal can be reinforced by the addition of WN. The WN frequencies, corresponding to the frequencies of the original signal, would resonate with each other and amplify the original signal without amplifying the rest of the WN, which results in an increase in the S/N ratio and makes the original sign more prominent.2727 Krauss P, Tziridis K, Metzner C, Schilling A, Hoppe U, Schulze H. Stochastic resonance controlled upregulation of internal noise after hearing loss as a putative cause of tinnitus-related neuronal hyperactivity. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:1-14. SR concept was first used by Pawel Jastreboff as one of the factors that leads to habituation of symptoms in tinnitus retraining therapy.1717 Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Tinnitus retraining therapy: a different view on tinnitus. ORL J Otorhinolaryngol Relat Spec. 2006;68:23-9. However, use the SR to explain the positive influence of WN on the performance of WM tests in our study, from an acoustical point of view, seems insufficient.
Schilling et al. (2021)2828 Schilling A, Tziridis K, Schulze H, Krauss P. The stochastic resonance model of auditory perception: A unified explanation of tinnitus development, Zwicker tone illusion, and residual inhibition. Prog Brain Res. 2021;262:139-57. hypothesized that SR regulates neural activity based on the well-known residual-inhibition phenomenon. The presence of external noise reduces neural activity, resulting in a decline in the perception of tinnitus. SR may explain how the brain processes inputs in noisy environments from individual synapses and single neurons to complete neural networks. Applying this reasoning model, it can be hypothesized that external WN stimuli may reduce neural activity, suppress tinnitus awareness, and free up additional cognitive resources for high WM task performance.
However, the effects of WN on WM remain unclear. There is no consensus in the literature, perhaps because of the variety of approaches used for these measurements. Although noise has often been considered a blurring factor for standard activeness, recent theoretical and experimental studies have shown a plausible constructive role, since it can improve neuronal firing steadiness and constancy in single and cluster neurons.2929 Ermentrout GB, Galán RF, Urban NN. Reliability, synchrony and noise. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:428-34. Othman et al. (2019)3030 Othman E, Yusoff AN, Mohamad M, Manan HA, Giampietro V, Hamid AIA, et al. Low intensity white noise improves performance in auditory working memory task: An fMRI study. Heliyon. 2019;5:e02444. studied the influence of WN on low WM in normal hearing. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) was used to understand how neural networks act while performing different S/N tasks. They observed a significant improvement, revealed by greater frontal, primary auditory, and anterior cingulate cortex activation in all noise conditions, except for the 0 dB S/N ratio. They concluded that the optimal S/N ratio to improve WM performance was between +10 and +5 dB. In our study, the most frequent S/N ratios among participants with better performance on noise WMAB were +30 and +15 dB, suggesting that tinnitus patients may need a higher S/N ratio to achieve SR benefits.
The MoCA test revealed that 48% of the participants had mild cognitive impairment and 20% showed low WM capacity (performance less than 3 spans).3131 Just MA, Carpenter PA. A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:122-49. Lee et al. (2020)3232 Lee J, Lee S, Han W, Kim J. Literature review of listening effort using subjective scaling. Korean J Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2017;60:99-106. studied MoCA-K (Korean version) in 58 participants aged >65 years and observed that 17.2% performed less than normal. They established a relationship between a THI ≥ 30 and low MoCA-K rating and hypothesized that tinnitus can negatively affect attentional orientation and executive control and reduce cognitive processing speed. The exact physiopathology underlying these findings has yet to be fully elucidated. In the present study, four patients aged ≥ 65 years had lower MoCA performance. However, we did not find any relationship between this and THI scores, perhaps due to the small sample size.
Psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and depression are prevalent in chronic tinnitus patients.1414 Waechter S, Hallendorf L, Malmstein E, Olsson A, Brännström KJ. The impact of tinnitus on n-back performance in normal hearing individuals. J Am Acad Audiol. 2019;30:169-77 Mild-to-moderate to severe depressive symptoms were observed in 52% of our sample, and anxiety symptoms were observed in 20%. A recent United States study of 21.4 million adults with tinnitus reported a prevalence of 25.6% of depression symptoms and 26.1% of anxiety symptoms.3333 Bhatt JM, Bhattacharyya N, Lin HW. Relationships between tinnitus and the prevalence of anxiety and depression. Laryngoscope. 2016;127:466-69. The difference between our study and the literature may be attributed to sample size.
A great variability in results was observed in the HFA; 44% of participants aged 51 years or older showed lower auditory thresholds. These findings are similar to those of Vielsmeier et al. (2015),3434 Vielsmeier V, Lehner A, Strutz J, Steffens T, Kreuzer PM, Schecklmann M, et al. The relevance of the high frequency audiometry in tinnitus patients with normal hearing in conventional pure-tone audiometry. Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015:302515. who observed HFA impairment among older adults, perhaps because of the greater sensitivity in detecting such changes. They also noticed a strong association between the worst thresholds in HFA and tinnitus laterality, which was not observed in the present study. A possible explanation is that 48% of our HFA auditory thresholds were close to normal compared to 17% of those in the aforementioned study. Four participants in our study had a complete lack of auditory perception bilaterally for HFA and poor performance on the WMAB; these data are similar to those reported by Waechter et al. (2019),1414 Waechter S, Hallendorf L, Malmstein E, Olsson A, Brännström KJ. The impact of tinnitus on n-back performance in normal hearing individuals. J Am Acad Audiol. 2019;30:169-77 who linked cognitive performance to the absence of auditory thresholds for HFA. A possible explanation may be the finding of Melcher et al. (2013),3535 Melcher JR, Knudson IM, Levine RA. Subcallosal brain structure: correlation with hearing threshold at supra-clinical frequencies (> 8 kHz), but not with tinnitus. Hear Res. 2013;295:79-86. who related a lower amount of gray matter (in the subcallosal area) to lower auditory thresholds for HFA. As the subcallosal area manages attentional processes, less grey matter may imply lower complex cognitive task performance. Patients with tinnitus may experience cognitive decline due to auditory threshold deterioration for HFA or due to the symptom itself, in which awareness can be more intrusive and bothersome for some participants.
In general, our findings regarding WM function are in agreement with part of the literature that reports a negative influence of tinnitus, which seems to increase cognitive load and decrease residual ability on task performance. Thus, during a WM task, voluntary effort is required for consciousness and control and fewer cognitive resources are required for good performance.77 Degeest S, Keppler H, Corthals P. The effect of tinnitus on listening effort in normal-hearing young adults: a preliminary study. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60:1036-45.,1111 Nagaraj MK, Bhaskar A, Prabhu P. Assessment of auditory working memory in normal hearing adults with tinnitus. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2020;277:47-54.,1212 Sharma A, Mohanty M, Panda N, Munjal S. Impact of hearing loss on cognitive abilities in subjects with tinnitus. Neurol India. 2022;70:554-62. There is still no consensus on a WM gold standard test. Clinical trials do not seem to control bias variables such as age, sex, hearing loss, education, fatigue, depression and anxiety symptoms, sleep pattern, and emotional motivation.
Our study revealed a relationship between higher THI scores and worse WMAB performance; however, since our sample was small and statistical significance was observed only in the mean analysis, this finding should be considered with caution. The data disagree with Nagaraj et al. (2020),1111 Nagaraj MK, Bhaskar A, Prabhu P. Assessment of auditory working memory in normal hearing adults with tinnitus. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2020;277:47-54. who did not show any correlation between the THI and high WM test responses. The authors suggested that their findings could not be generalized as the sample size studied was not significant. No similar studies have evaluated the effect of WN on high WM tests among participants with chronic tinnitus and normal hearing.
This study has limitations because the sample showed great variability in terms of age, cognition, and years of education. The presence of the control group could have contributed to a better understanding of the influence of white noise. However, such a group should be matched exactly with age, cognitive level, and education to avoid any bias. In addition, we cannot overlook the possible influence of personal motivation.3636 Peelle JE. Listening effort: how the cognitive consequences of acoustic challenge are reflected in brain and behavior. Ear Hear. 2018;39:204-14. during the test performance.
Conclusion
The presence of WN positively impacted WM performance in a subgroup of patients suggesting a release of cognitive resources (attention, learning, thinking, reasoning, remembering, problem solving, and decision-making, as described by the Cattel-Horn-Carroll Theory and Miyake taxonomy) and less auditory effort under these combined conditions. Further research must be performed to establish the most effective WN loudness for increasing WM skills on each case. At this point, we can reach a new rank in sound therapy practice for tinnitus management.
Acknowledgement
Our sincere gratitude to Dr Vitor Geraldi Haase and Guilherme Wood for sharing all WMAB (Working Memory Assessment Battery- Federal University of Minas Gerais) materials.
-
FundingThis research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
References
-
1Tyler RS, Baker LJ. Difficulties experienced by tinnitus sufferers. J Speech Hearing Disord. 1983;48:150-4.
-
2Goldstein B, Shulman A. Central auditory speech test findings in individuals with subjective idiopathic tinnitus. Int Tinnitus J. 1999;5:16-9.
-
3Pichora-Fuller MK, Kramer SE, Eckert MA, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear Hear. 2016;37(Suppl 1):S5-S27.
-
4Rönnberg J, Lunner T, Zekveld A, et al. The Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model: theoretical, empirical, and clinical advances. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013;7:31.
-
5Webb SL, Loh V, Lampit A, Bateman JE, Birney DP. Meta-analysis of the effects of computerized cognitive training on executive functions: A cross-disciplinary taxonomy for classifying outcome cognitive factors. Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28:232-50.
-
6Rossiter S, Stevens C, Walker G. Tinnitus and its effect on working memory and attention. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49:150-60.
-
7Degeest S, Keppler H, Corthals P. The effect of tinnitus on listening effort in normal-hearing young adults: a preliminary study. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60:1036-45.
-
8Gudwani S, Munjal SK, Panda NK, Kohli A. Association of chronic subjective tinnitus with neuro- cognitive performance. Int Tinnitus J. 2017;21:90-7.
-
9Tavanai E, Mohammadkhani G. A different view on the link between tinnitus and cognition; is there a reciprocal link? Int J Neurosci. 2018;128:1188-98.
-
10Wang Y, Zhang J-N, Hu W, Li J-J, 6 Zhou J-X, Zhang J-P, et al. The characteristics of cognitive impairment in subjective chronic tinnitus. Brain Behav. 2018(3):e00918.
-
11Nagaraj MK, Bhaskar A, Prabhu P. Assessment of auditory working memory in normal hearing adults with tinnitus. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2020;277:47-54.
-
12Sharma A, Mohanty M, Panda N, Munjal S. Impact of hearing loss on cognitive abilities in subjects with tinnitus. Neurol India. 2022;70:554-62.
-
13Mohamad N, Hoare DJ, Hall DA. The consequences of tinnitus and tinnitus severity on cognition: A review of the behavioural evidence. Hear Res. 2015;332:199-209.
-
14Waechter S, Hallendorf L, Malmstein E, Olsson A, Brännström KJ. The impact of tinnitus on n-back performance in normal hearing individuals. J Am Acad Audiol. 2019;30:169-77
-
15Waechter S, Wilson, WJ, Brännström JK. The impact of tinnitus on working memory capacity. Int J Audiol. 2021;60:274-81.
-
16Hobson J, Chisholm E, Refaie AE. Sound therapy (masking) in the management of tinnitus in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010;(12):CD006371.
-
17Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Tinnitus retraining therapy: a different view on tinnitus. ORL J Otorhinolaryngol Relat Spec. 2006;68:23-9.
-
18Sushun Z, Tyler R. A review of sound therapies for tinnitus. Ann Otolaryngol Rhinol. 2020;7:1233.
-
19Wood GMO, Carvalho MRS, Rothe-Neves R, Haase VG. Validation of the working memory assessment battery at UFMG (Validação da bateria de avaliação da memória de trabalho na UFMG). Psicol Refl Crít. 2001;14:325-41.
-
20World Health Organization. World report on hearing. March 3, 2021. Available at: https://www.who.int/teams/noncommunicable-diseases/sensory-functions-disability-and-rehabilitation/highlighting-priorities-for-ear-and-hearing-care Accessed June 29, 2023.
» https://www.who.int/teams/noncommunicable-diseases/sensory-functions-disability-and-rehabilitation/highlighting-priorities-for-ear-and-hearing-care -
21Henry JA, Zaugg TL, Schechter MA. Clinical guide for audiologic tinnitus management I: assessment. Am J Audiol. 2005;14:21-48.
-
22Salthouse TA, Babcock RL. Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:763-76.
-
23Sarmento ALR. Presentation and applicability of the Brazilian version of the MoCA for screening mild cognitive impairment. Master dissertation. Universidade Federal de São Paulo - Escola Paulista de Medicina; 2009. Accessed June 29, 20https://repositorio.unifesp.br/bitstream/handle/11600/8967/Publico-00377.pdf
» https://repositorio.unifesp.br/bitstream/handle/11600/8967/Publico-00377.pdf -
24Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-9 - validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Int Med. 2001;16:606-13.
-
25Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, Lowe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Intern Med. 2006;16:1092-7.
-
26Ferreira PEA, Cunha F, Onishi ET, Branco-Barreiro FCA, Ganança FF. Tinnitus handicap inventory: cultural modifications for Brazilian Portuguese (Tinnitus handicap inventory: adaptação cultural para o Português Brasileiro). Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2005;17:303-10.
-
27Krauss P, Tziridis K, Metzner C, Schilling A, Hoppe U, Schulze H. Stochastic resonance controlled upregulation of internal noise after hearing loss as a putative cause of tinnitus-related neuronal hyperactivity. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:1-14.
-
28Schilling A, Tziridis K, Schulze H, Krauss P. The stochastic resonance model of auditory perception: A unified explanation of tinnitus development, Zwicker tone illusion, and residual inhibition. Prog Brain Res. 2021;262:139-57.
-
29Ermentrout GB, Galán RF, Urban NN. Reliability, synchrony and noise. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:428-34.
-
30Othman E, Yusoff AN, Mohamad M, Manan HA, Giampietro V, Hamid AIA, et al. Low intensity white noise improves performance in auditory working memory task: An fMRI study. Heliyon. 2019;5:e02444.
-
31Just MA, Carpenter PA. A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:122-49.
-
32Lee J, Lee S, Han W, Kim J. Literature review of listening effort using subjective scaling. Korean J Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2017;60:99-106.
-
33Bhatt JM, Bhattacharyya N, Lin HW. Relationships between tinnitus and the prevalence of anxiety and depression. Laryngoscope. 2016;127:466-69.
-
34Vielsmeier V, Lehner A, Strutz J, Steffens T, Kreuzer PM, Schecklmann M, et al. The relevance of the high frequency audiometry in tinnitus patients with normal hearing in conventional pure-tone audiometry. Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015:302515.
-
35Melcher JR, Knudson IM, Levine RA. Subcallosal brain structure: correlation with hearing threshold at supra-clinical frequencies (> 8 kHz), but not with tinnitus. Hear Res. 2013;295:79-86.
-
36Peelle JE. Listening effort: how the cognitive consequences of acoustic challenge are reflected in brain and behavior. Ear Hear. 2018;39:204-14.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
23 Feb 2024 -
Date of issue
2024
History
-
Received
3 July 2023 -
Accepted
28 Sept 2023 -
Published
9 Oct 2023