Abstract
Aim: This narrative review aimed to explore the effect of mental fatigue on physical, technical, and tactical performance in ball sports.
Methods: Three Databases, PUBMED, SCOPUS, and SCIELO, were used to search for a scientific publication. The criterions adopted were: a) published in a peer-reviewed journal; b) adopted at least one manipulation check related to mental fatigue; c) mental fatigue induced by a cognitive task before the outcome task, and d) study participants were ball sports athletes.
Result: The quality of this narrative review was rated at 11 (SANRA scale 0-12), and the papers analyzed were published between 2015 and 2022. Twenty-one experimental studies were included in this review. Most studies were in invasion sports (n = 13; 72%), and non-ecological tasks were used to induce mental fatigue (n = 15; 83%). Regarding performance in endurance tests, mentally fatigued athletes had performance impaired. However, when assessed during a small-sided or simulated game, data were inconclusive within the literature. Moreover, athletes presented attenuated perceptual skills (e.g., visual field). Consequently, this worsened motor skills, technical-tactical performances, and tactical behavior.
Conclusions: Cognitively demanding tasks should be avoided before training and sports competitions because they can lead the athlete to mental fatigue and impair performance. This impairment is identified in physical performance (endurance tasks) and technical and tactical performance. Finally, it is recommended that athletes do not perform tasks that cause mental fatigue 2 h before the sports event.
Keywords cognitive neuroscience; cognitive fatigue; sport psychology; cognitive effort; athletic performance
Introduction
Ball sports (i.e., sports that use a ball or similar objects) are mainly determined by a holistic relationship between physical, technical, and tactical aspects1. Regarding the physical demands, intensity variation and brief rest periods (i.e., intermittent) are common in those modalities. Also, muscle power is essential to perform several actions (e.g., sprints, jumps, and kicks)1. In the technical (e.g., kick, header, forehand) and tactical (e.g., decisions and actions to gain an advantage over the opponent) aspects, perceptual-cognitive skills are required because most actions are open. In other words, they happen in an unpredictable environment, making it difficult to anticipate movements’ execution2. Therefore, executive function (i.e., mental processes that regulate attention, memory, and other cognitive skills; see more details in Diamond3) is fundamental to performance. For example, attentional control is responsible for the perception of relevant information, the decision making to find a better way of playing, and memory that directs attention and sustains elements that contribute to decision making4. In sports science, factors that impair performance are often investigated. For this reason, mental fatigue has garnered the interest of researchers, coaches, and athletes alike.
Conceptually, mental fatigue is a psychobiological state associated with a feeling of tiredness and/or lack of energy after long periods of cognitive activities that contain high levels of attention, inhibitory control, or monotony/boredom5,6. Using social media on smartphones7, playing video games8, and performing computational cognitive tests9,10 are examples of activities bringing about mental fatigue if performed for a prolonged period (i.e., ≥ 30 min). Additionally, contextual factors (e.g., traveling to compete) and cognitive demands during the match can also contribute to the increased perception of mental fatigue11.
One mechanism that seems to explain mental fatigue is the inability to resynthesize adenosine triphosphate (ATP) due to cognitive effort12. ATP (i.e., a combination of adenine, ribose, and a set of three phosphate derivatives, the oxidation of carbohydrate, protein, and lipids) is utilized during several human processes (e.g., muscle contraction, brain function)13. Blood glucose passes through the blood-brain barrier (i.e., formed by endothelial cells that select substances from blood into nervous tissue) and is regulated by the glucose transporter-1 (GLUT-1) membrane protein and hexokinase in a membrane-bound and intracellular fashion14. Glucose is converted to lactate in the brain, released into the extracellular space, and then transported to neurons and oxidized15.
Cognitive demand for a prolonged period requires a significant contribution from glucose for cerebral energy, which can be observed from the increase in lactate in extracellular regions. Although of short duration, the phosphocreatine (PCr) energy system appears to play an essential role in maintaining cognitive performance when the cerebral glycolytic system is overloaded16. When performing demanding cognitive activities, the demand for ATP surpasses the resynthesis capacity and, consequently, there is an increase in the concentration of cerebral adenosine, mainly in the anterior cingulate cortex6,17. It can be observed in situations of cerebral hypoxia, in which there is an increase in adenosine concentration and a decrease in ATP due to compromised oxygen supply18.
Adenosine increases are associated with reduced cortical activity19. For example, a systematic review with meta-analysis reported an increase in theta wave (4-7.5 Hz) in frontal regions and low-frequency alpha (7.5-13 Hz) in central cortical regions occurred in mentally fatigued subjects20, suggesting deactivation of brain areas. The practical effect is an impairment of executive function (e.g., working memory, attention)21 since these waves are associated with inhibitory processes, mainly in the frontal region, especially due to attenuation of dopamine activity, an excitatory neurotransmitter17.
It should be noted that the perceived exertion during exercise for the same external load is greater when the athlete is mentally fatigued22. It seems that perception of effort is decisive for exercise tolerance (for more details, see the psychobiological model)23. Moreover, executive function (executive control and perceptual speed) was associated with sports performance skills24, in which more skillful athletes exhibit superior cognitive function25. Thus, how mental fatigue affects executive functions and technical and tactical aspects might change depending on the individual6. Then, once players are expected to perform cognitive activities (e.g., social media, playing video games, school/university class) before training and matches (for more detail, see Thompson et al.11), it is crucial to investigate the effect of mental fatigue in ball sports.
Other reviews have attampted to shed light on the effect of mental fatigue on sports performance. Van Cutsem et al.26 examined the effect of mental fatigue on endurance, maximal strength, power, and anaerobic work. Recently, Cao et al.27 focused only on basketball, and Grgic et al.28 observed its effects on intermittent tests (i.e., Yo-Yo Test) and specific tests for football. Thus, a study to synthesize the information on mental fatigue and ball sports is absent to date, so we though it pragmatic to produce such an analysis. This review will assist coaches and other professionals responsible for athletes’ performance. This narrative review aimed to explore the effect of mental fatigue on physical, technical, and tactical performance in ball sports.
Methods
This article adopted “ball sports” as any sport played with a ball or similar object (e.g., badminton shuttlecock). A narrative review was performed using the databases PUBMED, SCOPUS, and SCIELO with the terms “mental fatigue” OR “cognitive fatigue” AND “sports” on the 11th of April 2022. In addition, a manual search by the title was performed in ResearchGate when the full version of the paper was not available in databases. Only papers that had access to the full version were taken for analysis. The criterions adopted were: a) published in a peer-reviewed journal; b) adopted at least one manipulation check related to mental fatigue (e.g., Stroop Test, visual analog scale - VAS); c) mental fatigue induced by a cognitive task (e.g., AX-CPT) before the outcome task; and d) being ball sports athletes [any person who is regularly training in sports, regardless of level (e.g., grassroots, amateurs, and professionals players) was considered].
Data analysis
The scientific publications were organized according to the type of sport, participants, and mental fatigue tasks. Moreover, this category is presented in an absolute and relative frequency distribution.
Result
The quality of this narrative review was rated at 11 (SANRA scale 0-12, see more details in supplementary files) using the Scale for the Assessment of Narrative Review Articles29, and the searched papers were published between 2015 and 2022.
Research on mental fatigue in ball sports and characteristics
Table 1 presents the 21 experimental studies included in this review. Following the Hughes and Bartlett30 classification, most research focused on invasion games (n = 16; 76.19%), mainly due to investigations with soccer players (n = 11; 66.67%). These studies were conducted primarily with male athletes, and only two studies (9.53%) had female participants. To induce mental fatigue (i.e., independent variable), most studies used the Stroop Test (n = 14; 66.67%). Furthermore, non-ecological activity (Stroop and AX-CPT) represented more than 79% (n = 15) of investigations. Only 23.81% (n = 5) considered ecological tasks, that is, activities potentially reproducible in a real-life scenario.
Furthermore, a series of tests and task configurations were used. In this way, Table 2 organizes these by sports. In this sense, it is observed that, except for football, most other sports invest in using physical tests. Finally, simulated matches with the same characteristics as official games are rarely used.
Discussion
Physical performance
Endurance tests
Ball sports involve intermittent exercise, so tests such as Yo-Yo Intermittent Recovery Test (Yo-Yo IR tests) are best suited to assess endurance capacity. Several authors have reported poorer Yo-Yo IR test performance following mental fatigue quantified by less distance covered22,31-33. The lower tolerance to exertion cannot be explained by physiological variables (i.e., heart rate, lactate), as these were not influenced by mental fatigue. For example, soccer athletes showed no difference between mental fatigue and control for heart rate22, which was later consolidated in handball athletes with no difference in blood lactate and heart rate recovery31. In another study using an intermittent treadmill test, mentally fatigued athletes reduced velocity at low intensities34. Thus, mental fatigue causes athletes to terminate tests earlier than control conditions because of increased subjective perception of effort (RPE)6.
Small-sided games and simulated match
In a small-sided soccer game (5 vs. 5) of 15 min, athletes covered the same distance between conditions (mental fatigue or control) when assessed by the global positioning system (GPS) but reported a greater perception of effort under mental fatigue35. Another study, also with soccer athletes, using another small-sided game configuration36, reported similar results. But the results observed by Coutinho et al.36 cannot be guaranteed that they were exclusively due to mental fatigue since the inducing task involved physical effort. Curiously, mentally fatigued soccer players covered greater distances during a small-sided game37, which may be explained by different tactical behaviors between conditions (Stroop Task vs. watched a video documentary). Finally, in a study with basketball (a small-sided game) and two other simulated soccer matches, the internal load (IL = RPE * time) was similar between conditions38. However, GPS or any other technique was not used to measure the external load. In addition, the number of participants and task configuration could be a confounding factor for analyzing the physical aspects directly in small-sided games or simulating matches (i.e., official model). For example, the physical demands are different in small games according to the game space and the number of participants. Furthermore, the subjective perception of the session39 should be used in future investigations. This method would facilitate the comparison between studies as it considers aspects of the internal (RPE) and external (time) load.
Strength, speed, and power tests
As a strength, speed, and power tests are of short duration, the subjective perception of effort has little importance in regulating performance in these tasks. Thus, in a study that verified torque production capacity in the elbow flexors and knee extensors, there was no reduction in the maximum voluntary contraction under mental fatigue, even though there was a tendency towards a greater subjective perception of effort9. Another study found lower limb power, quantified by vertical jumps with countermovement, and a specific test for badminton reported by the authors with a wide anaerobic characteristic were not impaired by mental fatigue40. Surprisingly, cricket athletes were slower on a modality-specific speed test32. However, this test had a technical component involved, which may explain the reduction in performance even though it is a short-term activity.
Technical and tactical performance
Technique in ball sports is usually investigated from a biomechanical perspective. However, for open skill sports, it is not overly relevant, as it cannot determine the result per se compared to predominantly closed skills sports, like sports with a greater technical emphasis (e.g., artistic gymnastics, rhythmic gymnastics). Moreover, in sports with a perceptual-cognitive aspect, tactics are of utmost importance for performance, as it relates to decisions and actions to gain an advantage over the opponent and manage the game space41,42. Thus, many studies have been developed to assess the effect of mental fatigue on perceptual-cognitive skills (response time, visual behavior, and decision making) and motor skills. These investigations were included in this section to infer the effect of mental fatigue on sports with technical-tactical aspects and tactical behaviors.
Perceptual-cognitive skills
In badminton, athletes were tested using a visual-motor task with target and distracting stimuli, athletes’ response times were slower when mentally fatigues43. Table tennis athletes also had attenuated performance (i.e., reaction time) on a slightly modally adapted visual-motor task44. In addition, the authors made an important contribution, indicating that mental fatigue modified the brain waves through the electroencephalogram. In this sense, decreases in the alpha band (upper: 4-8 Hz) and theta (10-13 Hz) were reported. Thus, in another study involving simple stimuli (specific test for badminton with four alternatives, without distractor), authors did not identify a negative effect of mental fatigue on performance40. These studies suggest that response time may be maintained in simple tasks, but in complex tasks, athletes require more time to develop an adequate motor response.
Two studies with soccer athletes demonstrated that mental fatigue causes visual changes. First, changes in the visual behavior of athletes were verified through eye-tracking45. In this study, there was a tendency for mentally fatigued athletes to focus more on the ball and defender than on free spaces and unmarked attackers. Secondly, there was a reduction in the visual field (i.e., a lower range of peripheral vision)37. This effect of mental fatigue on the visual field is important because it reduces the input of helpful information for decision making, therefore, attenuating tactical performance. For example, it was recently identified that soccer athletes with greater tactical efficiency also had greater peripheral perception46. Recently, this assumption was confirmed with basketball athletes fatigued by video games, showing changes in visual behavior (i.e., decreased number of fixations), compromising decision making (i.e., lower accuracy and more extended response time)47.
Furthermore, other studies also reinforce decision-making impairment in both controlled and open tasks. In soccer-specific decision-making tasks, soccer athletes exhibited poorer accuracy in determining the best option and response time in soccer-specific decision-making tasks45. The main limitation of this study is using a screen test, although real scenes were used. It differs from the requirements of a real match as motor actions were not necessary. However, this limitation extends to the previously presented study with basketball athletes47. In another study in which soccer athletes performed a simulated match with official rules, it was found that decision-making concerning passing was impaired under mental fatigue10. The authors demonstrated that 30 min of demanding cognitive activity was required to impair subsequent performance. Subsequently, these results were corroborated7,8 with two investigations as they incorporated mentally fatiguing activities likely to be completed by athletes (e.g., video games and social media). Thus, it demonstrates higher ecological validity than ‘artificial’ mentally fatiguing tasks (as athletes are unlikely to undertake the Stroop test in their leisure time or before a match). In addition, the data from these studies allow considering the visual, pre-motor, and supplementary motor brain areas as possibly affected by mental fatigue because they are directly related to perception and transformation of visual stimulus into a motor action48.
Motor skills
To assess passing and shooting skills in soccer players, Smith and colleagues used the Loughborough Soccer Passing Test, and the Loughborough Shooting Tests, respectively22. Data from this study revealed mental fatigue increased the number of penalties associated with the passing test and reduced speed and accuracy in shooting. More recently, using the same tests, mental fatigue was compared in three age groups (U-14, U-16, and U-18)33 with authors reporting a negative effect of mental fatigue in only U-18 athletes in the Loughborough Soccer Passing Test. It is possible younger athletes are less susceptible to mental fatigue due to less development of the prefrontal cortex49,50, however, more studies are needed in this regard. In a real match scenario, it is necessary to consider the opposition, but even in these ‘artificial’ tests discussed above, without the additional level of difficulty of facing opponents, accuracy was impaired which would be advantageous for opponents in retaking possession or not being threatened with shots at goal. Moreover, in net sports in which accuracy is highly important since error directly results in a point for the opponent, less accuracy and ball speed in table tennis were observed in mentally fatigued athletes9. Despite few studies, sports-specific motor skills seem to be affected by mental fatigue, but this area requires larger confirmatory studies.
Technical-tactical performance and tactical behavior
Mental fatigue seems to interfere with both technical-tactical performance and tactical behaviors. Mentally fatigued soccer players participated less during a small-sided game, missed more passes, lost control of the ball more easily, and had less success in dispossessing the ball of opposing player35. Similarly, mentally fatigued basketball players have been observed to make more turnovers under mental fatigue38. Two studies conducted by Kunrath and colleagues with young U-1551 and university soccer players37 assessed tactical behavior based on the principles of game and spatiality. Regardless of age, mentally fatigued athletes mainly neglected defensive principles. Furthermore, university players reduced collective tactical behavior and instead chose more individual actions in the attack.
Coutinho et al.36 reported mental fatigue affected athletes’ environmental perception sufficiently to harm their positioning on the field. For example, lateral synchronization was 13% lower than in a control condition without mental fatigue. In terms of cerebral activity, it is noteworthy that the anterior cingulate cortex appears directly related to a reduction in performance of tasks with technical-tactical and tactical behavioral demand as this area is functionally associated with attention, focus, and inhibitory control. However, it is still necessary to investigate whether mental fatigue acts in regions such as the cerebellum and all the “observation network” since they are fundamental areas for anticipation and refining movement52, a fundamental skill in ball sports.
In general, mental fatigue seems to impair all technical and tactical performance components. However, especially in “Motor skills” and “Technical-tactical performance and tactical behavior”, extrapolating the results between sports is challenging. This fact happens due to the variability of the task configuration. For example, even in a single sport like soccer, the configuration of the small-side games is quite distinct (more details in Table 2). It indicates that it is essential to investigate the specific demands of each sport because of the complex nature of sports. In addition, more investigations need to be done using the official rules of each sport.
Can motivation moderate mental fatigue?
In the model proposed by Ishii et al.53, mental fatigue is regulated by the facilitation and inhibition system (i.e., “double regulation system”), with motivation being a moderating factor for the overlap of one system over another53. Moreover, Martin et al.17 suggest that due to increased adenosine and reduction in dopaminergic activity, motivational aspects would be attenuated. The rationale is more motivated athletes could maintain performance even with an increase in the subjective perception of effort. However, investigations concerning motivation in ball sports are relatively limited. Studies carried out with badminton40, soccer22, and handball31 athletes did not identify any difference in motivation with the mentally fatigued athletes. However, these studies did not use a validated instrument for this purpose. To our knowledge, only one study evaluated motivation using a validated questionnaire (i.e., Assessment of motivational states in performance environments)34. Regardless, these results were similar to those utilizing an unvalidated assessment method, in that, there was no difference in motivation. The self-determination theory54 proposes three dimensions of motivation (i.e., intrinsic, extrinsic, and demotivation). The studies presented, regardless of the instrument seem to consider only the intrinsic motivational state. Thus, future investigations could investigate how extrinsic motivation moderates mental fatigue and subsequent performance.
Practical recommendations
It is recommended that coaches involved with ball sports guide athletes to avoid performing activities with high mental demands for more than 30 min, at least 2 h before training and competitions10. For example, electronic games and social networks on smartphones should be avoided8,11. It is essential because it prevents athletes from training or playing in a state of mental fatigue and consequently reduces their performance, especially the cognitive and technical-tactical ones. Moreover, it is widespread to use video before games to convey tactical information, and it should be used with caution. Only one study has shown that this type of activity for 30 min does not impair soccer athletes’ performance55.
In addition, monitoring of mental fatigue is indicated through the visual analog scale (10 cm - magnitude: 0-1, no fatigue; 2-4, mild fatigue, 5-7, moderate fatigue, 8-10, several fatigues)56,57, and short-term Stroop Test using accuracy and especially the response time as indicators7. These tools are accessible and easy to apply compared to an electrophysiological resource sensitive to mental fatigue such as an electroencephalogram58.
Limitations and future research
The results presented may be affected by the publication bias of the articles used, mainly due to the visual analog scale as a manipulation check (i.e., subjective measurements). Another important limitation of studies on mental fatigue is the task used (i.e., independent variables). Many studies use tasks like Stroop and AX-CPT tasks that are almost impossible to do in the real world before a match. However, there seems to be a concern in more recent studies to verify the effect of more ecological tasks. For example, Thompson et al.11, surveyed soccer young English athletes (i.e., U-14 to U-20), verifying that 2 h before the official games, the athletes used social networks (58%), used smartphones (12%), or played video games (9%), exactly the tasks used by Fortes et al.7,8. From this point forward, manipulation checks like VAS (e.g., subjective way) and Stroop task (i.e., behavioral way) can be used together, but an advance in this area would be the use of neurophysiological measures (i.e., electroencephalogram), and measure adenosine levels.
Recent studies have shown a positive effect of using caffeine to mitigate mental fatigue in endurance sports59. In the future, investigations with caffeine can clarify the mechanism related to adenosine, as it is a classic inhibitor, and an ergogenic aid for situations in which mental fatigue cannot be avoided (e.g., successive matches, travel). Moreover, strategies such as the use of transcranial direct current stimulation and mindfulness can also be tested to mitigate mental fatigue. Additionally, long-term studies with repeated effects need to be conducted to investigate the effects of mental fatigue on athletes’ preparation and consider the season and expand sports, categories, and genders.
Conclusions
In conclusion, mental fatigue appears to have harmful consequences on performance in ball sports (Figure 1). Regarding physical aspects, mental fatigue reduces endurance performance of periodic tests due to a greater perception of effort. During games (small-sided games or simulated), there is some inconsistency in findings, but mentally fatigued athletes choose to maintain RPE, thus worsening physical performance or reducing the physical effort to maintain RPE. Finally, perceptual-cognitive aspects are affected and contribute to worse performance in isolated tasks, small-side games, or matches. Consequently, it is recommended to avoid cognitively demanding activities 2 h before training and matches.
Acknowledgments
This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - Brasil (CAPES) - Finance Code 001.
References
-
1. Bangsbo J. Performance in sports - with specific emphasis on the effect of intensified training. Scand J Med Sci Sport. 2015;25(4):88-99. Doi: doi
» https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.12605 - 2. Gallahue DL. Classifying movement skills: a case for multidimensional models. J Phys Educ 2002;13(2):105-11.
-
3. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64(1):135-68. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 -
4. Afonso J, Garganta J, Mesquita I. Decision-making in sports: the role of attention, anticipation and memory. Rev Bras Cineantropometria e Desempenho Hum. 2012;14:592-601. doi
» https://doi.org/10.5007/1980-0037.2012v14n5p592 -
5. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J Appl Physiol. 2009;106(3):857-64. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008 -
6. Smith MR, Thompson C, Marcora, SM, Skorski S, Meyer T, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue and soccer: current knowledge and future directions. Sport Med. 2018;48(7):1525-32. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-018-0908-2 -
7. Fortes LS, Lima-Junior D, Nascimento-Júnior JRA, Costa EC, Matta MO, Ferreira MEC. Effect of exposure time to smartphone apps on passing decision-making in male soccer athletes. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;44(1):35-41. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.05.001 -
8. Fortes LS, Lima-Junior D, Fiorese L, Nascimento-Júnior JRA, Mortatti AL, Ferreira MEC. The effect of smartphones and playing video games on decision-making in soccer players: a crossover and randomised study. J Sports Sci. 2020;38(5):552-8. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2020.1715181 -
9. Le Mansec Y, Pageaux B, Nordez A, Dorel S, Jubeau M. Mental fatigue alters the speed and the accuracy of the ball in table tennis. J Sports Sci. 2018;36(23):2751-9. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2017.1418647 -
10. Gantois P, Ferreira MEC, Lima-Junior D, Nakamura FY, Batista GR, Fonseca FS, et al. Effects of mental fatigue on passing decision-making performance in professional soccer athletes. Eur J Sport Sci. 2019;20(4):534-43. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2019.1656781 -
11. Thompson CJ, Noon M, Towlson C, Perry J, Coutts AJ, Harper LD, et al. Understanding the presence of mental fatigue in English academy soccer players. J Sports Sci. 2020;38(13):1524-30. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2020.1746597 -
12. Dunwiddie TV, Masino SA. The role and regulation of adenosine in central nervous system. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2001;41(1):145-74. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.31 -
13. Bonora M, Patergnani S, Rimessi A, Marchi E, Suski JM, Bononi A, et al. ATP synthesis and storage. Purinergic Signal. 2012;8(3):343-57. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1007/s11302-012-9305-8 -
14. McAllister MS, Krizanac-Bengez L, Macchia F, Naftalin RJ, Pedley KC, Mayberg MR, et al. Mechanisms of glucose transport at the blood-brain barrier: an in vitro study. Brain Res. 2001;904(1):20-30. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02418-0 -
15. Dienel GA, Hertz L. Glucose and lactate metabolism during brain activation. J Neurosci Res. 2001;66(5):824-38. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.10079 -
16. Chen L, Schär M, Chan KWY, Huang J, Wei Z, Lu H, et al. In vivo imaging of phosphocreatine with artificial neural networks. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):1-10. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-14874-0 -
17. Martin K, Meeusen R, Thompson KG, Keegan R, Rattray B. Mental fatigue impairs endurance performance: a physiological explanation. Sport Med. 2018;48(9):2041-51. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-018-0946-9 -
18. Nieber K, Eschke D, Brand A. Brain hypoxia: effects of ATP and adenosine. In: Illes P, Zimmermann H. Nucleotides and their receptors in the nervous system. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam; 1999. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63563-3 -
19. Porkka-Heiskanen T. Adenosine in sleep and wakefulness. Ann Med. 1999;31(2):125-9. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3109/07853899908998788 -
20. Tran Y, Craig A, Craig R, Chai R, Nguyen H. The influence of mental fatigue on brain activity: evidence from a systematic review with meta-analyses. Psychophysiology. 2020;57(5):1-17. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13554 - 21. Herrmann CS, Strüber D, Helfrich RF, Engel AK. EEG oscillations: from correlation to causality. Int J Psychophysiol. 2016;103(1):12-21.
-
22. Smith MR, Coutts AJ, Merlini M, Deprez D, Lenoir M, Marcora SM. Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific physical and technical performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2015;48(2):267-76. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000762 -
23. Smirmaul BPC, Dantas JL, Nakamura FY, Pereira G. The psychobiological model: a new explanation to intensity regulation and (in)tolerance in endurance exercise. Rev Bras Educ Física e Esporte. 2013;27(2):333-40. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1590/s1807-55092013005000008 -
24. Trecroci A, Duca M, Cavaggioni L, Rossi A, Scurati R, Longo S, et al. Relationship between cognitive functions and sport-specific physical performance in youth volleyball players. Brain Sci. 2021;11(2):227. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11020227 - 25. Verburgh L, Scherder EJA, Van Lange PAM, Oosterlaan, J. Executive functioning in highly talented soccer players. PLoS One. 2014;9(3):e91254. doi
-
26. Van Cutsem J, Marcora SM, De Pauw K, Bailey S, Meeusen R, Roelands B. The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sport Med. 2017;47(8):1569-88. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0 -
27. Cao S, Geok SK, Roslan S, Sun H, Lam SK, Qian S. Mental fatigue and basketball performance: a systematic review. Front Psychol. 2022;12(1):819081. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.819081 -
28. Grgic J, Mikulic I, Mikulic P. Negative effects of mental fatigue on performance in the yo-yo test, loughborough soccer passing and shooting tests: a meta-analysis. J Funct Morphol Kinesiol. 2022;7(1):10. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3390/JFMK7010010 -
29. Baethge C, Goldbeck-Wood S, Mertens S. SANRA - a scale for the quality assessment of narrative review articles. Res Integr Peer Rev. 2019;4(1):5. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1186/s41073-019-0064-8 - 30. Hughes MD, Bartlett RM. The use of performance indicators in performance analysis. J Sports Sci. 2002;20(10):739-54.
-
31. Penna EM, Filho E, Campos BT, Pires DA, Nakamura FY, Mendes TT, et al. Mental fatigue does not affect heart rate recovery but impairs performance in handball players. Rev Bras Med do Esporte. 2018;24(5):347-51. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1590/1517-869220182405180483 -
32. Veness D, Patterson SD, Jeffries O, Waldron, M. The effects of mental fatigue on cricket-relevant performance among elite players. J Sports Sci. 2017;35(24):2461-67. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1273540 -
33. Filipas L, Borghi S, La Torre A, Smith, MR. Effects of mental fatigue on soccer-specific performance in young players. Sci Med Footb. 2021;5(2):150-7. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/24733938.2020.1823012 -
34. Smith MR, Marcora SM, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue impairs intermittent running performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2015;47(8):1682-90. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000592 -
35. Badin OO, Smith MR, Conte D, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue: impairment of technical performance in small-sided soccer games. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2016;11(8):1100-05. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1123/ijspp.2015-0710 -
36. Coutinho D, Gonçalves B, Travassos B, Wong DP, Coutts AJ, Sampaio JE. Mental fatigue and spatial references impair soccer players’ physical and tactical performances. Front Psychol. 2017;8(1):1645. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01645 -
37. Kunrath CA, Nakamura FY, Roca A, Tessitore A, Costa ITC. How does mental fatigue affect soccer performance during small-sided games? A cognitive, tactical and physical approach. J Sports Sci. 2020;38(15):1818-28. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2020.1756681 -
38. Moreira A, Aoki MS, Franchini E, Machado DGS, Paludo AC, Okano A. Mental fatigue impairs technical performance and alters neuroendocrine and autonomic responses in elite young basketball players. Physiol Behav. 2018;196(1):112-8. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.08.015 - 39. Foster C, Florhaug JA, Franklin J, Gottschall L, Hrovatin LA, Parker S, et al. A new approach to monitoring exercise training. J Strength Cond Res. 2001;15(1):109-15.
-
40. Kosack MH, Staiano W, Folino R, Hansen MB, Lønbro S. The acute effect of mental fatigue on badminton performance in elite players. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2020;15(5):632-8. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1123/ijspp.2019-0361 -
41. Kaya A. Decision making by coaches and athletes in sport. Procedia - Soc Behav Sci. 2014;152(1):333-8. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.09.205 - 42. Teoldo I, Manuel J, Greco PJ, Mesquita, I. Tactical principles of soccer game: concepts and application. Motriz: J Phys Edu. 2009;15(3):657-68.
-
43. Van Cutsem J, De Pauw K, Vandervaeren C, Marcora S, Meeusen R, Roelands B. Mental fatigue impairs visuomotor response time in badminton players and controls. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;45(1):101579. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101579 -
44. Habay J, Proost M, De Wachter J, Díaz‐García J, De Pauw K, Meeusen R, et al. Mental fatigue‐associated decrease in table tennis performance: is there an electrophysiological signature? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(24):12906. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182412906 -
45. Smith MR, Zeuwts L, Lenoir M, Hens N, De Jong LMS, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific decision-making skill. J Sports Sci 2016;34(14):1297-304. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1156241 -
46. Andrade MOC, Costa VT, García-Calvo T, Figueiredo A, Figueiredo A, Teoldo I. Peripheral perception as discriminant factor of tactical behaviour efficiency of young soccer players. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2021;19(6):1034-45. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2021.1948588 -
47. Fortes LS, Lima-Junior D, Barbosa BT, Faro HKC, Ferreira MEC, Almeida SS. Effect of mental fatigue on decision-making skill and visual search behaviour in basketball players: an experimental and randomised study. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2022;21(1)1-20. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2022.2058055 -
48. Hülsdünker T, Strüder HK, Mierau A. The athletes’ visuomotor system-cortical processes contributing to faster visuomotor reactions. Eur J Sport Sci. 2018;18(7):955-64. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2018.1468484 -
49. Arain M, Haque M, Johal L, Mathur P, Nel W, Rais A, et al. Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2013;9(1):449-61. doi
» https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S39776 -
50. Filipas L, Gallo G, Pollastri L, La Torre A. Mental fatigue impairs time trial performance in sub-elite under 23 cyclists. PLoS One. 2019;14(6): e0218405. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218405 -
51. Kunrath CA, Cardoso F, Nakamura FY, Teoldo I. Mental fatigue as a conditioner of the tactical and physical response in soccer players: a pilot study. Hum Mov. 2018;19(3):16-22. doi
» https://doi.org/10.5114/hm.2018.76075 -
52. Balser N, Lorey B, Pilgramm S, Naumann T, Kindermann S, Stark R, et al. The influence of expertise on brain activation of the action observation network during anticipation of tennis and volleyball serves. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8(1):568. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00568 -
53. Ishii A, Tanaka M, Watanabe Y. Neural mechanisms of mental fatigue. Rev Neurosci. 2014;25(4):469-79. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2014-0028 -
54. Adams N, Little TD, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):68-78. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-024-1042-6_4 -
55. Gianmarco C, Antonio T, Mauro M, Harald, T. A video-based tactical task does not elicit mental fatigue and does not impair soccer performance in a subsequent small-sided game. Sports. 2022;10(3):31. doi
» https://doi.org/10.3390/sports10030031 -
56. Smith MR, Chai R, Nguyen HT, Marcora SM, Coutts AJ. Comparing the effects of three cognitive tasks on indicators of mental fatigue. J Psychol Interdiscip Appl. 2019;153(8):759-83. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2019.1611530 -
57. O’Keeffe K, Hodder S, Lloyd A. A comparison of methods used for inducing mental fatigue in performance research: individualised, dual-task and short duration cognitive tests are most effective. Ergonomics. 2020;63(1):1-12. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2019.1687940 -
58. Tanaka M, Ishii A, Watanabesaaki Y. Effects of mental fatigue on brain activity and cognitive performance: a magnetoencephalography study. Anat Physiol. 2015;5:S4. doi
» https://doi.org/10.4172/2161-0940.s4-002 -
59. Franco-Alvarenga PE, Brietzke C, Canestri R, Goethel MF, Hettinga F, Santos TM, et al. Caffeine improved cycling trial performance in mentally fatigued cyclists, regardless of alterations in prefrontal cortex activation. Physiol Behav. 2019;204(1):41-8. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.02.009 -
60. Coutinho D, Gonçalves B, Wong DP, Travassos B, Coutts AJ, Sampaio J. Exploring the effects of mental and muscular fatigue in soccer players’ performance. Hum Mov Sci. 2018;58(1):287-96. doi
» https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2018.03.004
-
Associate Editor: Gustavo R. Mota0000-0002-6109-0658. Universidade Federal do Triângulo Mineiro, Uberaba, MG, Brazil. E-mail: grmotta@gmail.com.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
11 July 2022 -
Date of issue
2022
History
-
Received
15 Mar 2022 -
Accepted
10 May 2022