Abstract
We investigated the reactions of four bat species from four different lineages to UV light: Hipposideros armiger (Hodgson, 1835) and Scotophilus kuhlii Leach, 1821, which use constant frequency (CF) or frequency modulation (FM) echolocation, respectively; and Rousettus leschenaultii (Desmarest, 1820) and Cynopterus sphinx (Vahl, 1797), cave and tree-roosting Old World fruit bats, respectively. Following acclimation and training involving aversive stimuli when exposed to UV light, individuals of S. kuhlii and C. sphinx exposed to such stimuli displayed conditioned reflexes such as body crouching, wing retracting, horizontal crawling, flying and/or vocalization, whereas individuals of H. armiger and R. leschenaultii, in most cue-testing sessions, remained still on receiving the stimuli. Our behavioral study provides direct evidence for the diversity of cone-based UV vision in the order Chiroptera and further supports our earlier postulate that, due to possible sensory tradeoffs and roosting ecology, defects in the short wavelength opsin genes have resulted in loss of UV vision in CF bats, but not in FM bats. In addition, Old World fruit bats roosting in caves have lost UV vision, but those roosting in trees have not. Bats are thus the third mammalian taxon to retain ancestral cone-based UV sensitivity in some species.
Chiroptera; evolution; fear conditioning; UV vision
BEHAVIOR
Behavioral evidence for cone-based ultraviolet vision in divergent bat species and implications for its evolution
Xuan FujunI; Hu KailiangI; Zhu TengtengI; Racey PaulII; Wang XuzhongI; Sun YiI,III
IInstitute of Molecular Ecology and Evolution, Institutes for Advanced Interdisciplinary Research. East China Normal University, Zhongshan Road, Shanghai, 200062, China
IICenter for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter in Cornwall. Penryn, TR109EZ, UK
IIICorresponding author. Center for Circadian Clocks, Soochow University, Renai Road, Suzhou, 215123, China. E-mail: syixz524@hotmail.com
ABSTRACT
We investigated the reactions of four bat species from four different lineages to UV light: Hipposideros armiger (Hodgson, 1835) and Scotophilus kuhlii Leach, 1821, which use constant frequency (CF) or frequency modulation (FM) echolocation, respectively; and Rousettus leschenaultii (Desmarest, 1820) and Cynopterus sphinx (Vahl, 1797), cave and tree-roosting Old World fruit bats, respectively. Following acclimation and training involving aversive stimuli when exposed to UV light, individuals of S. kuhlii and C. sphinx exposed to such stimuli displayed conditioned reflexes such as body crouching, wing retracting, horizontal crawling, flying and/or vocalization, whereas individuals of H. armiger and R. leschenaultii, in most cue-testing sessions, remained still on receiving the stimuli. Our behavioral study provides direct evidence for the diversity of cone-based UV vision in the order Chiroptera and further supports our earlier postulate that, due to possible sensory tradeoffs and roosting ecology, defects in the short wavelength opsin genes have resulted in loss of UV vision in CF bats, but not in FM bats. In addition, Old World fruit bats roosting in caves have lost UV vision, but those roosting in trees have not. Bats are thus the third mammalian taxon to retain ancestral cone-based UV sensitivity in some species.
KEY WORDS: Chiroptera; evolution; fear conditioning; UV vision.
Vision is fundamentally important in the animal kingdom, for example in mate choice, communication, and foraging. Mammalian retinae have rod photoreceptors for night vision and cone photoreceptors for daylight and color vision. For color discrimination, most mammals possess two cone types with spectrally different visual pigments (opsins) that have absorption maxima at short wavelengths (Ss, blue or ultraviolet light) and middle/long wavelengths (M/Ls, green or red light). Although bats are the second largest order of mammals, the role of vision in their lives has seldom been considered because most have small eyes and it has long been hypothesized that an evolutionary "tradeoff" has occurred between their poor vision and their unique hearing ability associated with echolocation (SPEAKMAN 2001, ZHAO et al. 2009). Many studies have however investigated bats' visual orientation and acuity in the past decade (EKLOF et al. 2002, RYDELL & EKLOF 2003, HOLLAND et al. 2005). Behavioral experiments in dark-adapted conditions have revealed that a phyllostomid, Glossophaga soricina (Pallas, 1766), can detect UV light (WINTER et al. 2003) and has stimulated related research.
Sensitivity to UV light is achieved by photoreceptors in the eye that contain a class of visual cone pigments maximally sensitive to light at wavelengths <400 nm (HUNT et al. 2001). WANG et al. (2004) studied the M/L and S cone opsin genes from three bat species and suggested that the S opsin gene controlling sensitivity to UV light should retain its function. Our lab sequenced those opsin genes in 33 species with diverse sensory ecologies and found that the S opsin gene of bats had undergone a dramatic divergence among four lineages (ZHAO et al. 2009). Among the Yangochiroptera and Rhinolophoidea, gene defects in high-duty-cycle echolocators using constant frequencies (CF bats) should result in loss of UV vision, but not in those using low-duty-cycles (frequency modulated or FM bats). Among Old World fruit bats (Yinpterochiroptera) roosting in caves with low-ambient light, UV vision should be lost, but not in tree-roosting species. Based on the coevolved theory of vision and hearing of bats (SPEAKMAN 2001, ZHAO et al. 2009), the possible evolutionary sensory tradeoffs between shortwave vision and echolocation and changes in roosting ecology are consistent with the results of examination of S opsin among 28 species of bats from these four lineages using immunohistochemistry (MULLER & PEICHL 2005, 2006, MULLER et al. 2007, KIM et al. 2008, FELLER et al. 2009), though the exact spectral tuning of S opsin cannot be directly established. Recently, MULLER et al. (2009) employed corneal electroretinograms (ERGs) to corroborate the postulated UV spectral functions of the S opsin genes physiologically in the phyllostomids G. soricina and Carollia perspicillata (Linnaeus, 1758). However, we still cannot directly include bats among the mammalian taxa that retain ancestral cone-based UV vision, because ERGs merely detect the capacity of photoreceptors for perceiving UV light, and they may have a function unrelated to vision, such as circadian rhythm regulation (NEI et al. 1997) and/ or endocrine modulation (BRAINARD et al. 1994). Previous studies have only inferred that bats have the prerequisites for cone-based UV vision (MULLER et al. 2007, 2009) and no study has tested this capacity in the order Chiroptera. The search for UV vision in mammals has so far focused mainly on specialized cone types. WINTER et al. (2003) suggested that the possible rod-modulation (beta-band) mechanism for UV light perception in the bats' eye was still unknown while for other mammals it remained a possibility.
Because their retinas typically contain only a relatively small population of cones (ca 3% in bats with laryngeal echolocation, even less than 0.5% in Old World fruit bats, MULLER & PEICHL 2005, 2006, MULLER et al. 2007, KIM et al. 2008), it is difficult to study the nature of cone-based vision in nocturnal mammals (JACOBS et al. 2001). Following methods used for the demonstration of possible cone-based pathways (JACOBS et al. 1991, AMIR & ROBINSON 1996, JACOBS et al. 2001, ARRESE et al. 2006, PALACIOS et al. 2010), only behavioral studies can confirm proposed UV vision (JACOBS 1993, GRIEBEL & PEICHL 2003). Classical fear conditioning occurs when an affectively neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus CS) is paired with a noxious aversive stimulus (unconditioned stimulus US) such as footshock (WALKER et al. 2002). This method could enlarge the neutral stimulus and has been successfully used in numerous species (from snails to humans, SANGHA et al. 2003, KINDT et al. 2009). Here we modified fear conditioning procedures to elucidate the bats' capacity for cone-based UV vision, since it is now well established that bats have functional visual systems and the corresponding anatomical evidence shows that both their cornea and lens can transmit UV light (MULLER et al. 2007, 2009). Our experiment was carried out under light conditions to avoid possible rod interference. Based on our molecular data mentioned above, the aim of the present investigation is to test our hypotheses that CF bats such as Hipposideros armiger (Hodgson, 1835) (Hipposideridae) and cave-roosting Old World fruit bats such as Rousettus leschenaultii (Desmarest, 1820) (Pteropodidae) have lost UV vision, and no longer react to UV light stimuli, but FM bats such as Scotophilus kuhlii Leach, 1821 (Vespertilionidae) and tree-roosting Old World fruit bats such as Cynopterus sphinx (Vahl, 1797) (Pteropodidae) have retained UV vision. The results will also indicate the diversity of cone-based UV vision in the order Chiroptera and provide further insight into the evolutionary history of visual capacities of diverse bat species.
MATERIAL AND METHODS
Adult S. kuhlii, R. leschenaultii and C. sphinx were captured in Hainan Province, China, and H. armiger was captured in Yulong Cave, Anhui Province, China. The bats of each species were housed together in an individual mesh cage (80 × 80 × 80 cm) in a ventilated temperature-controlled (24ºC) feeding room (5 × 6 × 3 m). In the daytime (12:12 h light/dark cycle), the transmitting light provided by two fluorescent lamps (18W, Philips, Guangdong, China) was around 100 lx (measured with a calibrated luxmeter, Shenzhen Sampo Instrument Co., Shenzhen, China) in the inner cages. With ad libitum access to fresh water and weekly vitamin supplements, the two bat species with laryngeal echolocation were offered mealworms and two Old World fruit bat species were offered apples and/or bananas. The experiments were conducted at least three weeks after the animals were captured to ensure that they had acclimated to their new environment. In this study, the use of animals was approved by the Animal Experimental Ethical Committee of East China Normal University and followed its ethical guidelines for the care and use of animals.
Six bats of each species were tested. The experiment was operated in a ventilated wooden chamber (1 × 1 × 1 m) (Figs 1 and 2), the ceiling of which contained a square door hinged on one side. The floor insulation provided a temporary refuge for the bats especially when we were selecting and adjusting stimulus time and intensities during the training phases. The internal sidewalls were covered with twelve 5 cm-wide horizontal copper net bands with a three cm interval between them, from which the animals could cling and complete electrical circuits during the training and cue-testing. The metal bands were wired to a hand-operated shock scrambler and generator. Ultraviolet light was provided by a 6W UV hand lamp (365 nm, 20 nm half bandwidth, Spectroline, NY, USA), the radiant energy 15 cm away from which was 900 µm/cm2. Before experiments, the lamp was placed on the floor of the chamber. Four infrared cameras inside the chamber were placed diagonal to one other, two on the roof and two on the floor so that almost all of the chamber could be monitored. A computer that ran Super-DVR software (DVR4Net PCI card) and interfaced with the cameras recorded events inside the chamber in real time. The wooden chamber was placed in the feeding room, adjacent to the bats' cages. The door was opened and together with the ventilation holes in the sidewalls provided persistent light illumination of around 100 lx during daytime experimental procedures. The bats thus experienced similar environmental conditions during behavioral experiments and possible interference from other environmental cues except UV light (CS) was reduced.
Behavioral procedures consisted of an acclimation phase, a training (fear-conditioning) phase, and a cue-testing (extinction) phase. In the acclimation phase, bats were introduced individually into the chamber for approximately 30 minutes per day for a week prior to the training phase to reduce the stress associated with handling and acquainting them to the chamber, which they explored by crawling and/or resting on the metal bands. Each day animals were handled at different times of day (09.00-16.00 h) to ensure that they did not entrain to handling by the experimenter at a specific time.
On the day of training, bats were placed individually into the chamber and the experiments did not begin until the bats showed no obvious activity. Then the UV lamp was placed opposite to the bat. Each bat received 10 CS-US trials. A 0.4 mA, 1 sec shock was delivered after a 3 sec UV light stimulus (around 25 µm·cm-2, measured with a power meter, Physcience Opto-Electronics Co., Beijing). The stimulus parameters used in the experiment strictly complied with the detailed behavioral protocols for examining the nature and properties of fear extinction in laboratory rodents (CHANG et al. 2009), as bats are comparable in size to mice or rats. As soon as they suffered from footshock, each individual vocalized and flew, showing acute responses. The average inter-trial interval was three minutes (range, 2-5 min). During these intervals, we frequently adjusted the positions of the lamps with respect to the bats to ensure that the eyes of bats could perceive the UV light. FANSELOW (1980) pointed out that the distinct CRs observed immediately after shocks were similar to those after a delay of 24 hours. So in this phase, we gradually established the criteria for CRs in the bats (CS, 3 sec; 3 min inter-trial interval), as this was the first time that fear conditioning had been attempted in the order Chiroptera. For S. kuhlii and C. sphinx, the training procedures were carried out and persisted for about one week until distinct CRs were observed and stabilized. Conditioned reflexes were quantified by reflexes such as body crouching, wing retracting, horizontal crawling and/or vocalization, and flying as soon as the bats received the CSs. However, for H. armiger and R. leschenaultii, after two weeks training, we were still unable to observe distinct CRs. Since fear conditioning is a form of one-trial learning (FANSELOW 1990), here we could assume a robust and long-lasting behavioral change had been produced after these days of training among all individuals of four species.
For cue-testing, animals were returned individually to the same conditioning chamber. Followed the training procedures, each individual was continually exposed to a total of 100 UV light CSs (3 sec; 3 min inter-trial interval) in the following two or three days. However, due to the repeated presentation of CS in the absence of the US, the conditioning response would be reduced (fear extinction, CHANG et al. 2009). During this process, each 10 CSs ended with one CS-US trial to strengthen the effect of conditioning and the behaviors of each individual, whether reflex or not when they received UV light only under light conditions, were recorded. Lastly, we calculated the conditioned reflex rates for each species by averaging the positive activities (CRs) per 100 CSs among six individuals and due to limited independent samples for each species, we also applied Mann-Whitney U-tests for pairwise comparisons between species.
RESULTS
During the training phase, each individual of all four bat species experiencing CS-US trials showed the distinct acute reflexes of vocalizations and flying. But in most of the sessions, H. armiger and R. leschenaultii did not show obvious CRs in the absence of US. As soon as they received UV light, the bats kept still, merely shaking their wings slightly or occasionally crawling along the metal-net bands. They also crouched and sometimes even flew, especially during the first two or three training days. Therefore, we temporarily categorized them as the group testing negative while the other two species, S. kuhlii and C. sphinx tested positive because they clearly showed fear and had distinct CRs once UV light was delivered: body crouching, wing retracting, horizontal crawling and/or vocalization and flying. In one to two weeks of training, we assumed CRs of all individuals were strengthened and stabilized and arrived at the optimal stimulus parameters.
In the cue-testing phase, we observed a total of 100 UV light CSs for each individual. The maximum value for conditioned reflex rates for individual H. armiger and R. leschenaultii (testing negative) was 20 far less than the lowest (n = 91) achieved for S. kuhlii and C. sphinx (testing positive). The average conditioned reflex rates and the statistical pairwise comparisons among the four species are shown in Fig. 3: the two testing groups were clearly different (e.g. H. armiger vs S. kuhlii, U = 0, p < 0.01; R. leschenaultii vs C. sphinx, U = 0, p < 0.01) while within groups there was no difference (e.g. H. armiger vs R. leschenaultii, U = 9, p > 0.05; S. kuhlii vs C. sphinx, U = 8.5, p > 0.05).
DISCUSSION
Our behavioral study in four species among four lineages of bats found clear differences in the reactions of divergent bat species to UV light stimuli and provides direct evidence to suggest the diversity of cone-based UV vision in the order Chiroptera. It also supports previous molecular postulates from our own and other laboratories (WANG et al. 2004, MULLER & PEICHL 2005, 2006, MULLER et al. 2007, KIM et al. 2008, FELLER et al. 2009, ZHAO et al. 2009).
Among bats with laryngeal echolocation, H. armiger, as a member of the Hipposideridae-Rhinolophidae CF lineage, has lost the capacity for cone-based UV vision (no obvious CRs) but can utilize Doppler shifts, the most advanced nocturnal sensory adaptation within mammals, to receive and process a more continuous flow of acoustic information (HIRYU et al. 2005, JONES 2005). In contrast, S. kuhlii, belonging to the FM group, including all members of the Yangochiroptera and independently the Yinpterochiropteran taxon Megaderma spasma (Linnaeus, 1758) (JONES & TEELING 2006), has retained UV vision (evident CRs), which presumably can be used for orientation and/or hunting. Our previous work presented evidence of independent losses of shortwave opsin functionality early in the evolution of the Hipposideridae-Rhinolophidae lineage according to the phylogenetic and molecular evolutionary analyses of S opsin genes in bats, indicating a possible evolutionary sensory tradeoff between vision and hearing in these two lineages (ZHAO et al. 2009). Combined with our current work, it seems that CF bats, with the evolution of a novel form of echolocation, may have rendered dichromatic color vision redundant and lost it, whereas FM echolocators can augment their acoustic "image" with UV vision. In Old World fruit bat lineages, the cave-roosting R. leschenaultii has lost cone-based UV vision (no obvious CRs) while the tree-roosting C. sphinx has retained it (evident CRs). This indicates that loss of UV vision may be correlated with roosting ecology, i.e., different levels of ambient light. Tree-roosting species at exposed daytime roosts appear to require UV vision to aid their visual discrimination for predator surveillance (MULLER et al. 2007) while cave-roosting species may not require color discrimination and experience a relaxation in selective constraint of the S opsin gene, ultimately resulting in loss of UV vision (ZHAO et al. 2009).
Following consecutive acclimation and training, we made UV light the unique cue to produce the obvious CRs in S. kuhlii and C. sphinx under light conditions. This differed from the approach of WINTER et al. (2003) who gave bats a non-illuminated stimulus in dark-adapted conditions. They reported a possible rod-modulation mechanism, because at that time, due to technical limitations, bats, as the group of mammals best adapted to the nocturnal aerial feeding niche, were still mostly considered to lack cones. In view of bats' low light tolerance (HOPE & BHATNAGAR 1979, FURE 2006), any chromatic adaptation they adopted might render the intensity of stimulus provided too weak to show that they possess a separate UV light receptor as do rodents (JACOBS et al. 1991, JACOBS & DEEGAN 1994). In addition, we can rule out the possible interference of rods and avoid the corresponding contradiction between the photoreceptor data indicating cone monochromacy and the behavioral data indicating some capacity for color vision, as in whales and seals (GRIEBEL & PEICHL 2003). Because the secondary absorption peaks of "cis" from rods are more sensitive than those in cones to short wavelengths (GOURAS 1984), under mesopic lighting conditions, residual color vision in these cone monochromats could be achieved by exploiting the signal differences between the remaining green cones (L/M cones) and the rods. However, our experiment occurred in the daytime and rods could be bleached under such bright light conditions (PEPPERBERG 2003). Other than possible contextual interference (CHANG et al. 2009), H. armiger and R. leschenaultii exhibited no more reflexes during most cue-testing sessions although they had rod photorecepters but lacked functional S cone photorecepters. Meanwhile, the recent finding that cones were never blinded by light (LOBANOVA et al. 2010), might rationalize our procedures and the bleaching effect could not interrupt the cone-based visual pathway in bats, i.e. if H. armiger and R. leschenaultii possess functional S cones, they should also exhibit distinct CRs, as did S. kuhlii and C. sphinx under the same light conditions.
Until now the exact rod-based (beta-band) mechanism for UV vision in mammals has not previously been demonstrated (WINTER et al. 2003). However, recent electrophysiological investigations revealed that the rods of bats could perceive partial UV light with cones under low mesopic light conditions (0.03 lx, MULLER et al. 2009). Therefore, to judge whether there are two mechanisms acting simultaneously to modulate UV vision of bats, further research is required.
In conclusion, our work widens our understanding of the evolution of cone-based visual capacities in the Chiroptera and the fact that some species retain cone-based UV vision means that bats are the third mammalian taxon to do so (JACOBS et al. 1991, JACOBS et al. 2001, ARRESE et al. 2006, PALACIOS et al. 2010). To further our work, corresponding field investigations are now also required.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank David E. Lucero and other anonymous reviewers for valuable comments, Pan Yxuan for technical advice, and Zhu Guanjian and Zhang Junpeng for help in the laboratory. This study was supported by grants under the Key Construction Program of the National "985" Project and "211" Project to S.Z. and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31101641) to Y.S.
LITERATURE CITED
Submitted: 08.X.2011; Accepted: 29.II.2012.
Editorial responsibility: Kleber Del Claro
References
- AMIR, S. & B. ROBINSON. 1996. Fos expression in rat visual cortex induced by ocular input of ultraviolet light. Brain Research 716: 213-218.
- ARRESE, C.A.; L.D. BEAZLEY & C. NEUMEYER. 2006. Behavioural evidence for marsupial trichromacy. Current Biology 16: 193-194.
- BRAINARD, G.C.; F.M. BARKER; R.J. HOFFMAN; M.H. STETSON; J.P. HANIFIN; P.L. PODOLIN & M.D. ROLLAG. 1994. Ultraviolet regulation of neuroendocrine and circadian physiology in rodents. Vision Research 34: 1521-1533.
- CHANG, C.H.; E. KNAPSKA; C.A. ORSINI; C.A. RABINAK; J.M. ZIMMERMAN & S. MAREN. 2009. Fear extinction in rodents. Current Protocols in Neuroscience. 47: 8.23.1-8.23.17.
- EKLOF, J.; T. TRANEFORS & L.B. VAZQUEZ. 2002. Precedence of visual cues in the emballonurid bat Balantiopteryx plicata Mammalian Biology 67: 42-46.
- FANSELOW, M.S. 1980. Conditioned and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science 15: 177-182.
- FANSELOW, M.S. 1990. Factors governing one-trial contextual conditioning. Animal Learning & Behavior 18: 264-270.
- FELLER, K.D.; S. LAGERHOLM; R. CLUBWALAL; M.T. SILVER; D. HAUGHERY; J.M. RYAN; E.R. LOEW; M.E. DEUTSCHLANDER & K.L. KENYON. 2009. Characterization of photoreceptor cell types in the little brown bat Myotis lucifugus (Vespertilionidae). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology B-Biochemistry & Molecular Biology 154: 412-418.
- FURE, A. 2006. Bats and lighting. The London Naturalist 85: 1-20.
- GOURAS, P. 1984. Progress in human visual evoked responses. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 1: 77-82.
- GRIEBEL, U. & L. PEICHL. 2003. Colour vision in aquatic mammalsfacts and open questions. Aquatic Mammals 29: 18-30.
- HIRYU, S.; K. KATSURA; L.K. LIN; H. RIQUIMAROUX & Y. WATANABE. 2005. Doppler-shift compensation in the Taiwanese leaf-nosed bat (Hipposideros terasensis) recorded with a telemetry microphone system during flight. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 118: 3927-3933.
- HOLLAND, R.A.; P. WINTER & D.A. WATERS. 2005. Sensory systems and spatial memory in the fruit bat Rousettus aegyptiacus Ethology 111: 715-725.
- HOPE, G.M. & K.P. BHATNAGAR. 1979. Effect of light adaptation on electrical responses of the retinas of four species of bats. Experientia 35: 1191-1193.
- HUNT, D.M.; S.E. WILIE; J.K. BOWMAKER & S. POOPALASUNDARAM. 2001. Vision in the ultraviolet. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences 58: 1583-1598.
- JACOBS, G.H. 1993. The distribution and nature of colour vision among the mammals. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 68: 413-471.
- JACOBS, G.H. & J.F. DEEGAN. 1994. Sensitivity to ultraviolet light in the gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus): characteristics and mechanisms. Vision Research 34: 1433-1441.
- JACOBS, G.H.; J. NEITZ & J.F. DEEGAN. 1991. Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light. Nature 353: 655-656.
- JACOBS, G.H.; J.A. FENWICK & G.A. WILLIAMS. 2001. Cone-based vision of rats for ultraviolet and visible lights. Journal of Experimental Biology 204: 2439-2446.
- JONES, G. 2005. Echolocation. Current Biology 15: 484-488.
- JONES, G. & E.C. TEELING. 2006. The evolution of echolocation in bats. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21: 149-156.
- KIM, T.J.; Y.K. JEON; J.Y. LEE; E.S. LEE & C.J. JEON. 2008. The photoreceptor populations in the retina of the greater horseshoe bat Rhinolophus ferrumequinum Molecules and Cells 26: 373-379.
- KINDT, M.; M. SOETER & B. VERVLIET. 2009. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience 12: 256-258.
- LOBANOVA, E.S.; R. HERRMANN; S. FINKELSTEIN; B. REIDEL; N.P. SKIBA; W.T. DENG; R. JO; E.R. WEISS; W.W. HAUSWIRTH & V.Y. ARSHAVSKY. 2010. Mechanistic basis for the failure of cone transducin to translocate: why cones are never blinded by light. Journal of Neuroscience 30: 6815-6824.
- MULLER, B. & L. PEICHL. 2005. Retinal cone photoreceptors in microchiropteran bats. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 46: 2259.
- MULLER, B. & L. PEICHL. 2006. Cone photoreceptor diversity in the retinae of flying foxes (Megachiroptera). FENS Abstracts 3: A072.014.
- MULLER, B.; S.M. GOODMAN & L. PEICHL. 2007. Cone photoreceptor diversity in the retinas of fruit bats (Megachiroptera). Brain Behavior and Evolution 70: 90-104.
- MULLER, B.; M. GLOSMANN; L. PEICHL; G.C. KNOP; C. HAGEMANN & J. AMMERMULLER. 2009. Bat eyes have ultraviolet-sensitive cone photoreceptors. PLoS One 4: e6390.
- NEI, M.; J. ZHANG & S. YOKOYAMA. 1997. Color vision of ancestral organisms of higher primates. Molecular Biology and Evolution 14: 611-618.
- PALACIOS, A.G.; F. BOZINOVIC; A. VIELMA; C.A. ARRESE; D.M. HUNT & L. PEICHL. 2010. Retinal Photoreceptor Arrangement, SWS1 and LWS Opsin Sequence, and Electroretinography in the South American Marsupial Thylamys elegans (Waterhouse, 1839). Journal of Comparative Neurology 518: 1589-1602.
- PEPPERBERG, D.R. 2003. Bleaching desensitization: background and current challenges. Vision Research 43: 3011-3019.
- RYDELL, J. & J. EKLOF. 2003. Vision complements echolocation in an aerial-hawking bat. Naturwissenschaften 90: 481-483.
- SANGHA, S.; A. SCHEIBENSTOCK & K. LUKOWIAK. 2003. Reconsolidation of a long-term memory in Lymnaea requires new protein and RNA synthesis and the soma of right pedal dorsal 1. Journal of Neuroscience 23: 8034-8040.
- SPEAKMAN, J. 2001. The evolution of flight and echolocation in bats: another leap in the dark. Mammal Review 31: 111-130.
- WALKER, D.L.; K.J. RESSLER; K.T. LU & M. DAVIS. 2002. Facilitation of conditioned fear extinction by systemic administration or intra-amygdala infusions of D-cycloserine as assessed with fear-potentiated startle in rats. Journal of Neuroscience 22: 2343-2351.
- WANG, D.Y.; T. OAKLEY; J. MOWER; L.C. SHIMMIN; S. YIM; R.L. HONEYCUTT; H. TSAO & W.H. LI. 2004. Molecular evolution of bat color vision genes. Molecular Biology and Evolution 21: 295-302.
- WINTER, Y.; J. LOPEZ & O. VON HELVERSEN. 2003. Ultraviolet Vision in a Bat. Nature 425: 612-614.
- ZHAO, H.B.; S.J. ROSSITER; E.C. TEELING; C.J. LI; J.A. COTTON & S.Y. ZHANG. 2009. The evolution of color vision in nocturnal mammals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106: 8980-8985.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
07 May 2012 -
Date of issue
Apr 2012
History
-
Received
08 Oct 2011 -
Accepted
29 Feb 2012