Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Executive functions assessment in patients with language impairment A systematic review

Avaliação das funções executivas em pacientes com comprometimento de linguagem: uma revisão sistemática

Abstract

Acquired language impairments may accompany different conditions. Most recent studies have shown that there is an important relationship between language and cognitive functions, such as executive functions (EF). Therefore, we aimed to investigate which main EF components appear to have the greatest impact in the most prevalent acquired communication disorders in adults, and which neuropsychological tests are being used to evaluate them. In addition, we sought to characterize the relationship between the executive functions and language in these conditions. Working memory (WM) was the most frequently chosen cognitive measure, being evaluated by different span tasks. A relationship between WM and narrative and conversational discourse, writing abilities and grammatical comprehension was found. Other currently used cognitive tests included the Trail Making, Wisconsin, Stroop and Verbal Fluency tests. Language and EF have a complex relationship; hence, a complete assessment should reflect the dynamic processing of cognitive brain functions.

Key words:
executive functions; acquired language impairment; neuropsychological assessment

Resumo

Distúrbios de linguagem adquiridas podem acompanhar diferentes quadros. Estudos recentes mostram que existe uma relação importante entre linguagem e funções cognitivas, como as funções executivas (FE). Portanto, visou-se investigar quais os principais componentes das FE parecem ter um maior impacto nos quadros com transtornos de comunicação adquiridos mais prevalentes em adultos e quais testes neuropsicológicos estão sendo mais utilizados para avaliá-los. Além disso, buscamos caracterizar qual a relação entre as funções executivas e a linguagem nesses quadros. A memória de trabalho (MT) foi a medida cognitiva mais frequentemente citada, sendo avaliada por diferentes tarefas de Span. Encontrou-se relação entre MT e discurso narrativo e conversacional, habilidades de escrita e compreensão gramatical. Outros testes cognitivos usados incluem os testes Trail Making, Wisconsin, Stroop e Verbal Fluency. Linguagem e EF têm uma relação complexa e, portanto, uma avaliação completa desses pacientes deve refletir o dinâmico processamento do funcionamento cognitivo.

Palavras-chaves:
funções executivas; distúrbios de linguagem adquiridos; avaliação neuropsicológica

Several neurological and psychiatric disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease, primary progressive aphasia, post-stroke aphasia, traumatic brain injury and schizophrenia, can be accompanied by impairments in different language domains,11 Verma M, Howard RJ. Semantic memory and language dysfunction in early Alzheimer's disease: a review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27(12):1209-17.

2 Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, Cappa SF, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76(11):1006-14.

3 Constantinidou F, Wertheimer JC, Tsanadis J, Evans C, Paul DR. Assessment of executive functioning in brain injury: Collaboration between speech-language pathology and neuropsychology for an integrative neuropsychological perspective. Brain Inj. 2012;26:13-4.

4 Koehler C, Gindri G, Mancopes R. demência avaliados com a Bateria MAC Language alterations in elderly patients with dementia assessed with the MAC Battery. 2012;17(1):15-22.

5 Jamadar S, Powers NR, Meda SA, Calhoun VD, Gelernter J, Gruen JR, et al. Genetic influences of resting state fMRI activity in language-related brain regions in healthy controls and schizophrenia patients: A pilot study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2013;7(1):15-27.

6 Togher L, McDonald S, Tate R, Rietdijk R, Power E. The effectiveness of social communication partner training for adults with severe chronic TBI and their families using a measure of perceived communication ability. Yeates G, editor. NeuroRehabilitation. 2016;38(3):243-55.
-77 El Hachioui H, Visch-Brink EG, de Lau LML, van de Sandt-Koenderman MWME, Nouwens F, Koudstaal PJ, et al. Screening tests for aphasia in patients with stroke: a systematic review. J Neurol. 2017;264(2):211-20. including phonological, lexical, semantic, prosodic, discursive, syntactic aspects. These language impairments affect quality of life, autonomy, prognosis, social relationships and increase patient and caregiver burden.88 Ferris SH, Farlow M. Language impairment in alzheimer's disease and benefits of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8:1007-14.-99 Arrighi HM, Neumann PJ, Lieberburg IM, Townsend RJ. Lethality of Alzheimer disease and its impact on nursing home placement. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(1):90-5. Some clinical conditions are known for having acquired language impairments, such as post-stroke aphasia, dementia, right-brain damage (RBD) and traumatic brain injury (TBI).

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia, responsible for 55% of all dementia cases.1010 Alzheimer Association. 2013 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimer's Dement. 2013;9(2):208-45. Even in its prodromal and early stages, it is already possible to identify changes in language abilities, mainly in the temporal characteristics of spontaneous speech, such as speech tempo, number of pauses in speech, and their length,1111 Szatloczki G, Hoffmann I, Vincze V, Kalman J, Pakaski M. Speaking in Alzheimer's disease, is that an early sign? Importance of changes in language abilities in Alzheimer's disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:1-7. in addition to naming disorders, impaired auditory and written comprehension, and semantic paraphasia.1212 Appell J, Kertesz A, Fisman M. A study of language functioning in Alzheimer patients. Brain Lang. 1982;17(1):73-91. On the other hand, repetition abilities and articulation seem to be relatively intact.1313 Croot K, Hodges JR, Xuereb J, Patterson K. Phonological and articulatory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: A case series. Brain Lang. 2000;75(2):277-309. Different patterns of language impairment can be observed as the disease progresses.1414 Kempler D. Neurocognitive Disorders in Aging. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2004.

Aphasia can be defined as the inability to comprehend and/or formulate language because of damage to specific brain regions.1515 Damasio AR. Aphasia. N Engl J Med. 1992 Feb 20;326(8):531-9. Around 20% of patients who suffer a stroke are affected by aphasia.1616 Davidson B, Howe T, Worrall L, Hickson L, Togher L. Social Participation for Older People with Aphasia: The Impact of Communication Disability on Friendships. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2008;15(4):325-40. Post-stroke aphasia may occur at the phonetic, syntactic, semantic or pragmatic level of language processing. Aphasia has a heterogeneous presentation and can range from only an occasional difficulty in word-finding to losing the ability to speak, read, or write.1717 Stahl B, Van Lancker Sidtis D. Tapping into neural resources of communication: formulaic language in aphasia therapy. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1526.

Unlike left- brain damage, RBD does not present clear and widespread failures of language comprehension or extreme difficulty producing fluent speech. Fundamental word and sentence processing abilities are relatively unaffected by RBD.1818 Johns CL, Tooley KM, Traxler MJ. Discourse Impairments Following Right Hemisphere Brain Damage: A Critical Review. Lang Linguist Compass. 2008;(6):1038-62. On the other hand, patients after RBD may present difficulties in discourse comprehension and higher-level language tasks related to semantic and lexical processing.1919 Myers P. Right Hemisphere Damage: Disorders of Communication and Cognition. Norwich: Singular; 1999. In addition, they can also present limitations relating to figurative cues in language, as they tend to understand sentences from their literal meanings.2020 Lundgren K, Brownell H. Figurative Language Deficits Associated With Right Hemisphere Disorder. Perspect ASHA Spec Interes Groups. 2016;1(2):66. Finally, most TBI patients also have relatively spared lower-level language skills, in contrast to post-aphasia patients. However, they have difficulties in more complex/high-order language tasks, such as discourse.2121 Coelho CA. Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Semin Speech Lang. 2007;28(2):122-35. In addition, some patients may experience difficulties interpreting non-verbal signals such as body language and emotional signals, as well as prosodic dysfunction.

For several years, language impairments were considered isolated impairments from other cognitive domains. However, recent studies have highlighted the relationship between language domains and other cognitive functions, such as mnemonic, attentional and executive,2222 Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short term memory, working memory,and syntactic comprehension in Aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109.,2323 Mayer JF, Murray LL. Measuring working memory deficits in aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2012;45(5):325-39. and their importance for therapy outcomes.33 Constantinidou F, Wertheimer JC, Tsanadis J, Evans C, Paul DR. Assessment of executive functioning in brain injury: Collaboration between speech-language pathology and neuropsychology for an integrative neuropsychological perspective. Brain Inj. 2012;26:13-4. For this reason, it is important to take into account cognitive performance with associations and dissociations among attention, memories, executive abilities and different linguistic components when assessing and planning interventions for patients with language disorders. Both assessment and rehabilitation are influenced by the inter-relationship of language and other cognitive domains regarding their processing.

One of the most studied cognitive abilities are the executive functions (EF), the most complex and high-order neuropsychological components responsible for deliberating control of goal-oriented actions as well as for cognitive energy distribution.2424 Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex "Frontal Lobe" Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100. In the literature, there are several established models that outline these components.2525 Collins A, Koechlin E. Reasoning, Learning, and Creativity: Frontal Lobe Function and Human Decision-Making. PLoS Biol. 2012;10(3):e1001293.

26 Lehto JE, Juujärvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br J Dev Psychol. 2003;21(1):59-80.

27 Lunt L, Bramham J, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Selway RP, Xenitidis K, et al. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and "Jumping to Conclusions": Bias or deficit? J Neuropsychol. 2012;6(1):65-78.
-2828 Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex "Frontal Lobe" Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100. Among the most recent theoretical proposals, the model proposed by Diamond2929 Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-68. holds there are three core EF (inhibition, working memory and cognitive flexibility). These abilities are important for different life domains, such as work and school, mental and physical health, cognitive, social, and psychological development. EF are also central to other cognitive functions, such as language.

Deficits in executive functions can result in a reduced ability to organize thought and therefore language expression, where such impairment can lead to the production of irrelevant utterances, word-finding problems, impaired sequence at the word and propositional level, tangential language and verbosity leading to problems in communication abilities.3030 McDonald S, Togher L, Code C. Social and Communication Disorders Following Traumatic Brain Injury. 2nd ed. McDonald S, Togher L, Code C, editors. Social Communication Disorders Following Traumatic Brain Injury. New York: Routledge; 2013. EF are necessary for a successful conversation, such as being able to retain what was said by the other person, to plan a response, and if necessary, to inhibit an inappropriate response, relying on different processes such as working memory, planning, and inhibition.3131 Fridriksson J, Nettles C, Davis M, Morrow L, Montgomery A. Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clin Linguist Phon. 2006;20(6):401-10.

Pragmatic deficits are related to difficulties in communicating effectively in interaction contexts. More specifically, these deficits and EF components of working memory, verbal planning, initiation and inhibition, switching, shifting and strategy maintenance seem to have an association.3232 Zimmermann N, Gindri G, Oliveira CR de, Fonseca RP. Pragmatic and Executive Functions in Traumatic Brain Injury and Right Brain Damage. Dement Neuropsychol. 2011;5(4):337-45. Meanwhile, several variables of conversional discourse are related to cognitive flexibility, inhibition, verbal and visuospatial planning and processing speed.3333 Chabok SY, Kapourchali SR, Leili EK, Saberi A, Mohtasham-Amiri Z. Effective factors on linguistic disorder during acute phase following traumatic brain injury in adults. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(7):1444-50.

34 Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48.

35 Turkstra LS. Conversation-based assessment of social cognition in adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2008;22(5):397-409.
-3636 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93. These relationships between language and EF may manifest in patients with an acquired language disorder and can be relevant for assessment and rehabilitation programs.

The relationship between language and EF has been studied in healthy subjects. The more complex the language ability, the greater the recruitment of EF. These functions are recruited in a variety of language skills. For example, in order to tell a story, EF skills are required, such as planning and organization, otherwise the story may lack structure or important details. For auditory comprehension, it is necessary to inhibit distractions, while keeping the important information in working memory. Also, for reading comprehension, one needs to sustain attention on the text while keeping the information in working memory, integrating world knowledge with the new information acquired. Cognitive flexibility may help the reader to understand the text, even though they may not recognize a few words. These relationships also occur in individuals with different brain lesions, however, this may not take place in the most fluid way possible. The higher the pathological severity, the more EF will be necessary for language expression, in an attempt to compensate for the deficit in linguistic processing.

Although the relationship between linguistic and other cognitive dimensions has been increasingly studied, this interaction approach may be very challenging, mainly due to the fact that mnemonic, attentional, executive tasks may assess cognitive domains by means of linguistic stimuli, that is, there is underlying linguistic processing and/or production demands.3737 Helm-Estabrooks N. Cognition and aphasia: a discussion and a study. J Commun Disord. 2002;35(2):171-86. Also, language batteries are already extensive and adding a complete cognitive assessment would require numerous sessions for assessment, making it difficult for speech therapists to conduct both assessments. Also, an overly long assessment process can be very stressful for the patient. An accurate diagnosis of which functions are impaired, in addition to the identification of interference of a specific deficit in other functions, will increase the specificity of treatment plans.

To the best of our knowledge, there is no other systematic review of clinical tools targeting neurocognitive assessment of EF in patients with language impairment. Therefore, we aimed to investigate which main EF components may cause the greatest impact in language disorders, as well as the relationship itself, considering the most prevalent clinical conditions associated with acquired language impairments in adults, including post-stroke aphasias, traumatic brain injury, dementias and right-brain damage. In addition, we sought to investigate which neuropsychological tests are being used to evaluate which domains.

METHODS

For the selection of the abstracts, the Pubmed database was used together with the Prisma method for this article. The inclusion criteria for selecting the abstracts were: [1] empirical and clinical articles, [2] written in English, [3] published between 2000 and 2015, [4] with sample composed by adults with Alzheimer’s disease, traumatic brain injury, post-stroke aphasia or right- brain damaged patients [5] that had at least one executive component specifically assessed or at least a whole general executive function battery applied, and [6] discussed the relationship between language and at least one executive function. The present study also only included patients with a single brain lesion.

The keywords used were the following: executive functions OR inhibition OR inhibitory control OR interference control OR cognitive flexibility OR mental flexibility OR set shifting OR working memory OR central executive AND acquired communication disorders OR language impairment OR language disorder OR language deficits AND right-hemisphere damage OR right brain damage OR aphasia OR traumatic brain injury OR Alzheimer’s disease OR frontal temporal dementia OR dementia AND cognitive assessment OR cognitive screening OR neuropsychological assessment OR cognitive evaluation OR neuropsychological evaluation.

The exclusion criteria for the articles were only language assessment without at least one executive component (working memory, cognitive flexibility or inhibitory control) specifically assessed or at least a whole general executive function battery applied; rehabilitation and review articles; samples that included conditions other than Alzheimer’s disease, traumatic brain injury, post-stroke aphasia and right brain damaged patients.

Three independent judges analyzed 800 abstracts considering the inclusion and exclusion criteria of the present study. Initially, 28 abstracts were excluded because the related article was written in a language other than English and 33 involved children. 197 abstracts did not include one of the studied conditions (Alzheimer’s disease, traumatic brain injury, post-stroke aphasia and right brain damage). Reviews accounted for 27 dismissed abstracts. In addition, 7 rehabilitation articles were excluded. Other abstracts did not explore the relationship between language and at least one EF component and were therefore excluded. Finally, 107 articles, that had the concordance of at least 2 judges, were analyzed by the main author. However, after reading these articles, we only selected the 29 articles addressing the relationship between language and EF for discussion.

RESULTS

The main results will be presented by pathology, mentioning the main neuropsychological tests used.

Figure 1
Article selection process.

In addition, the relationship between EF and language found in each article is explored. The results are presented by clinical condition because their linguistic features can vary considerably.

Right brain damage

Only two articles with right brain damage (RBD) were selected. The first article, by McDonald,3838 McDonald S. Exploring the cognitive basis of right-hemisphere pragmatic language disorders. Brain Lang. 2000;75(1):82-107. employed two verbal tests, since tools that rely on visuospatial skills are not appropriate for RBD. These two tests were the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) for assessing generativity and rule compliance and the WAIS-R Similarities subtest (WAIR-S) for assessing verbal conceptual abilities. The second article, by Zimmermann et al.,3939 Zimmermann N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal Fluency in Right Brain Damage: Dissociations Among Production Criteria and Duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8. evaluated patients with RBD by verbal fluency for three different production criteria (unconstrained, phonemic, and semantic). Performance of verbal fluency tasks seem to rely on a basic set of cognitive processes, such as sustained attention, search strategy, inhibition, and working memory.4040 Birn RM, Kenworthy L, Case L, Caravella R, Jones TB, Bandettini PA, et al. of Verbal Fluency. 2011;49(1):1099-107.

This first article sought to investigate the hypotheses that difficulties in communication reported in RBD patients would reflect a loss of executive control secondary to damage to the frontal systems of the brain or their connections.3838 McDonald S. Exploring the cognitive basis of right-hemisphere pragmatic language disorders. Brain Lang. 2000;75(1):82-107. However, EF were not significantly associated with pragmatic competence in production or reception in this study. The reasons explaining why the executive dysfunction was unable to predict general pragmatic performance is unclear but could be related with the unilateral nature of the brain damage experienced. This study also showed that, for the executive impairments to disrupt pragmatic language skills, they may need to be particularly pervasive, or perhaps of a particular kind.3838 McDonald S. Exploring the cognitive basis of right-hemisphere pragmatic language disorders. Brain Lang. 2000;75(1):82-107.

Zimmerman et al.,3939 Zimmermann N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal Fluency in Right Brain Damage: Dissociations Among Production Criteria and Duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8. found that a larger number of patients with RBD had impairments in semantic verbal fluency, more associated with semantic memory retrieval than with phonemic or unconstrained verbal fluencies. A greater number of patients were detected using a longer task, when compared to a shorter version, which indicate more difficulty maintaining lexical search over time than actual executive impairments. Therefore, RBD patients seem to rely less on EF during verbal fluency tasks.3939 Zimmermann N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal Fluency in Right Brain Damage: Dissociations Among Production Criteria and Duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8.

Traumatic brain injury

Working memory was the EF component most studied in TBI articles.3434 Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48.,4141 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19(12):1001-9.

42 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Early conversational discourse abilities following traumatic brain injury: An acute predictive study. Brain Inj. 2014;28(7): 951-8.

43 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.

44 Douglas JM. Relation of Executive Functioning to Pragmatic Outcome Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365.

45 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.
-4646 Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46. The three main EF tests used were the Trail Making Test,4242 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Early conversational discourse abilities following traumatic brain injury: An acute predictive study. Brain Inj. 2014;28(7): 951-8.,4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.,4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.,4747 Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury?: An exploratory study using the GALI ( a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange ) §. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.,4848 Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9. Span3434 Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48.,4141 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19(12):1001-9.

42 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Early conversational discourse abilities following traumatic brain injury: An acute predictive study. Brain Inj. 2014;28(7): 951-8.
-4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285. and Stroop4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.,4747 Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury?: An exploratory study using the GALI ( a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange ) §. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.,4848 Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9. tasks. The Trail Making test was used to assess cognitive flexibility, a complex attention and planning. Articles did not specify for which EF component the Stroop task was used. Finally, Span tasks were used to assess working memory abilities.

Studies that sought to investigate the relationship between EF and discourse production found modest significant correlations between measures of WM and narrative discourse.4949 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. 2005;19:1001-9. These findings suggest that the story retelling task placed a greater demand on WM by requiring information processing and temporary storage of the information necessary for accurate retelling.4949 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. 2005;19:1001-9. However, Digits Backwards, a task usually used to assess WM, did not prove to be a significant predictor of pauses produced between clauses between speakers with TBI.4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285. On the other hand, the test of Likeness-Differences provides a verbal measure of EF and was able to predict the number of mazes produced per utterance. These findings suggest that microlinguistic deficits can be explained by deficits in the way individuals with TBI recruit and control attention for sentence planning.4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.

In addition, it was found that the poorer the performance on the executive tests, the more communication difficulties were present.5050 Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: An exploratory study using the GALI (a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83. Several specific items of a discursive test were found to be correlated with EF measures among TBI patients in the acute care phrase. Early conversational discourse in the acute care phase post-TBI is significantly related to WM, cognitive flexibility, divided attention, and initiation abilities.3636 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93. However, only a weak relationship was found between WM measures and inference production. Also, the TBI and control groups did not differ on the measure of proportion of explanations and predictions that relied on the maintenance of information in WM as the source.4646 Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46. Thus, it is unlikely that group differences in WM can explain the poorer narrative comprehension and tendency to focus more on local, sentence by sentence, detail in the TBI group.4646 Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46.

Table 1
Articles included in review.

Approximately one third of variability in the pragmatic problems reported was accounted for by measures of EF. Performance on a phonemic verbal fluency task was the only task able to predict pragmatic impairment.4444 Douglas JM. Relation of Executive Functioning to Pragmatic Outcome Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365. There was also a pattern of association between the ability to maintain information over time, performance and pragmatic competence. These findings suggest that impaired storage and retrieval processes can contribute to problems of relevance including topic management in conversation.4444 Douglas JM. Relation of Executive Functioning to Pragmatic Outcome Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365. EF integrity seems to be generally necessary for a successful conversational interchange to be maintained. However, unlike verbal communication, non-verbal communication did not seem to be related to any EF test.4848 Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9.

In a study with penetrating head injury, IQ was highly correlated with the cognitive variables of EF, WM and immediate memory, and by having these highly correlated variables as predictors of discourse measures in the model, the paths between IQ and cognitive ability were considered to be potentially redundant. IQ seems to account for cognitive ability, story completeness, and story grammar.3434 Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48. A group with penetrating head injury had impairment in WM and immediate memory, possibly explaining the patients’ difficulty with completeness and story grammar, which may reflect their difficulty developing a mental representation of the story they saw and were asked to retell. The retelling was incomplete and disorganized, because of an inability to process the story, a lack of foundation or inefficient mapping.3434 Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48.

Finally, a study that sought to investigate semantic memory in TBI patients based on their capacity to learn new words that had recently entered the French language found correlation between the processing of new words from 2006 to 2007 and patients’ performance in the letter fluency and vocabulary tests.4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22. The difficulty in defining new words is probably related to difficulties retrieving lexical knowledge and in EF.4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.

Dementia

The most used EF test for assessing patients with dementia was the different modalities of digit spans used to assess WM abilities. For evaluating general EF performance, the phonemic and semantic verbal fluencies and the Trail Making Test were used. The Hayling Test was used in two studies for assessing inhibitory control.

Overall fluency impairment in small vessels disease seemed to be related to EF and processing speed deficits, common in this condition. These patients have relatively preserved semantic memory, so do not have an added disadvantage in semantic fluency tasks.5151 Herbert V, Brookes RL, Markus HS, Morris RG. Verbal Fluency in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease and Alzheimer's Disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(4):413-21 On the other hand, the AD group presented a discrepancy between phonological and semantic fluency, which may be explained by the greater reliance of semantic fluency tasks on semantic memory, which is degraded in AD.5151 Herbert V, Brookes RL, Markus HS, Morris RG. Verbal Fluency in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease and Alzheimer's Disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(4):413-21

Regarding sentence production, there seems to be a strong correlation between performance on WM tests and the sentence-repetition task. The strongest correlations were found in the sentence types with embedded relative clauses. However, correlations that involved Active and Passive sentences were less robust.5252 Small J a, Kemper S, Lyons K. Sentence repetition and processing resources in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2000;75:232-58. Tasks that demanded more WM had the strongest correlations.5252 Small J a, Kemper S, Lyons K. Sentence repetition and processing resources in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2000;75:232-58. However, a reduction in WM capacity was not associated with a decreased ability to devote processing resources to syntactic analysis and to the use of syntax to determine sentence meaning.5353 Waters G, Caplan D. Working memory and online syntactic processing in Alzheimer's disease: studies with auditory moving window presentation. journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(4):P298-311. In addition, strong relationships were not evident between the reverential communication task scores and inhibitory control mechanisms in the early stage of AD.5454 Feyereisen P, Berrewaerts J, Hupet M. Pragmatic skills in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease: An analysis by means of a referential communication task. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42:1-17. On the other hand, familiar idioms need suppression of the literal interpretation in order to for idiomatic meaning to be derived. This suppression in mediated by the central executive which may be impaired in AD patients, therefore the inhibition of the literal meaning is not efficient and the figurative meaning does not get sufficient activation.5656 Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95. Also, a close relationship between WM and language was identified, evidenced by the fact that complex skills placing demands on working memory (i.e., digit reversal, spelling, and calculation) did not place demands on the memory or the executive factor, but rather on a general language factor.5555 Stopford CL, Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D. Distinct Memory Profiles in Alzheimer's Disease. Cortex. 2007;43(7):846-57.

Perspective-taking performance was related with reasoning skills, the integrity of semantic memory, management of attention and response conflict.5656 Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95. Regarding writing skills, the number of correct responses in a writing task was related with working memory, immediate recall, inhibition and planning.5757 Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn H-J, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23(8):1317-26. Finally, metaphor comprehension seems to be more related to EF, than language skills themselves.5555 Stopford CL, Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D. Distinct Memory Profiles in Alzheimer's Disease. Cortex. 2007;43(7):846-57.

Aphasia

WM appears to be the most studied EF in aphasia patients, but there were some studies with different kinds of attentional systems and the EF of inhibition, planning, cognitive flexibility, working memory, sustained attention, perceptual tracking, and graphomotor skills. WM was measured using the backward visual memory span subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale and the digits backwards of the WAIS-III, in addition to other spans tasks such as Alphabet Span, Subtract-2 Span, and Sentence Span.

Better scores on short-term memory tests were associated with both experimental and baseline sentences. The dissociations found between normal performance in sentence comprehension and below normal performance on short-term memory (STM) indicates that normal STM is not necessary needed to support either parsing and interpretation per se or the use of STM in mapping interpretations onto operations required to perform tasks.5858 Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short-term memory, working memory, and syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109.

Passive sentences were more difficult to understand compared to active (as indicated by lower processing scores), and longer sentences were more difficult than shorter ones. These results were expected, given that comprehension deficits are characteristic of aphasia, and that individuals with aphasia tend to have difficulties understanding sentences with noncanonical thematic role orders such as passives. Neither complexity nor length of sentence by itself impacted recall.5959 Ivanova M V., Hallowell B. A new modified listening span task to enhance validity of working memory assessment for people with and without aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2014;52:78-98. The ability to switch between processing and storage in the WM span task is the primary influence on WM capacity indices, not the difficulty of the task or characteristics of the linguistic stimuli.5959 Ivanova M V., Hallowell B. A new modified listening span task to enhance validity of working memory assessment for people with and without aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2014;52:78-98.

Patients with aphasia present impairments in visual-spatial WM and difficulties in generalization and abstraction on non-verbal material.6060 Seniów J, Litwin M, Lesniak M. The relationship between non-linguistic cognitive deficits and language recovery in patients with aphasia. J Neurol Sci. 2009;283(1-2):91-4. After a rehabilitation program, an improvement was observed in two important language functions, naming and comprehension, that was associated with patients’ baseline non-verbal visual-spatial working memory.6060 Seniów J, Litwin M, Lesniak M. The relationship between non-linguistic cognitive deficits and language recovery in patients with aphasia. J Neurol Sci. 2009;283(1-2):91-4.

A correlation with grammatical comprehension deficit and limited WM capacity can also be seen in progressive non-fluent Aphasia.6161 Grossman M, Moore P. A longitudinal study of sentence comprehension difficulty in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76(5):644-9. However, this correlation seems to be specific for grammatically complex sentences, given there were no significant correlations with grammatically simple sentences.6161 Grossman M, Moore P. A longitudinal study of sentence comprehension difficulty in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76(5):644-9. Aphasia severity symptoms were associated with patient performance on WM measures, for both verbal and spatial WM scores.6262 Potagas C, Kasselimis D, Evdokimidis I. Short-term and working memory impairments in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2874-8.

A correlational and regression analyses showed significant relationships between participants’ attention deficits and their language and communication performance. The more complex attention skills had a stronger correlation with language and communication. It also was found that attention allocation difficulties negatively affected auditory comprehension and spoken language.6363 Murray LL. Attention and other cognitive deficits in aphasia: Presence and relation to language and communication measures. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2012;21(2):51-65.

For some aphasic patients, executive functioning may be a better indicator of functional communication ability when compared to language impairment.3131 Fridriksson J, Nettles C, Davis M, Morrow L, Montgomery A. Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clin Linguist Phon. 2006;20(6):401-10. Decreased fluency was also associated with a decline in functional communication ability. These results may indicate that a decrease in fluency is a greater barrier to functional communication than impaired auditory comprehension.3131 Fridriksson J, Nettles C, Davis M, Morrow L, Montgomery A. Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clin Linguist Phon. 2006;20(6):401-10.

However, another study evaluating conversional abilities found no correlation between cognitive impairments and the severity of spoken language impairments.6464 Knibb JA, Woollams AM, Hodges JR, Patterson K. Making sense of progressive non-fluent aphasia?: an analysis of conversational speech. 2009;2734-46. Another study also failed to find an association between language and WM skills. These findings could be explained by the fact that the simple language tasks requiring basic linguistic operations and direct retrieval of lexical items from the semantic system, such as naming of objects, single word comprehension, single word repetition, and production/comprehension of short and simple sentences, do not rely heavily on WM.2323 Mayer JF, Murray LL. Measuring working memory deficits in aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2012;45(5):325-39. Perhaps, with more complex language tasks, particularly tasks targeting receptive language abilities, an association can be found between WM and language measures.2323 Mayer JF, Murray LL. Measuring working memory deficits in aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2012;45(5):325-39.

DISCUSSION

Language skills are essential for successful social interaction in different domains such as personal relationships and the work environment.6565 Hebert-Myers H, Guttentag CL, Swank PR, Smith KE, Landry SH. The Importance of Language, Social, and Behavioral Skills Across Early and Later Childhood as Predictors of Social Competence With Peers. Appl Dev Sci. 2006;10(4):174-87. Linguistic impairments may be a reflection of deficits between cognition and linguistic processing, rather than a specific difficulty of language.6666 Marini A, Galetto V, Zampieri E, Vorano L, Zettin M, Carlomagno S. Narrative language in traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2904-10.

Considering the importance of further elucidating the relationship of executive functioning and language impairments, the aim of this article was to define which EF components were the most present when assessing patients with language disorder and which neuropsychological tests were chosen for their assessment. Also, the study sought to investigate the relationship between EF and language in the most prevalent acquired communication disorders among adults.

WM was the cognitive measure most frequently chosen in the articles of this review. It was present in studies of TBI,4949 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. 2005;19:1001-9. dementia5656 Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95. and aphasia.5858 Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short-term memory, working memory, and syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109. Different span tasks were used to assess WM, such as alphabet span,5858 Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short-term memory, working memory, and syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109. sentence span5858 Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short-term memory, working memory, and syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109. and a modified version span.5959 Ivanova M V., Hallowell B. A new modified listening span task to enhance validity of working memory assessment for people with and without aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2014;52:78-98. Digit span was the most commonly used cognitive test to evaluate WM, present in several studies.4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.,4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.,4949 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. 2005;19:1001-9.,5252 Small J a, Kemper S, Lyons K. Sentence repetition and processing resources in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2000;75:232-58.,5353 Waters G, Caplan D. Working memory and online syntactic processing in Alzheimer's disease: studies with auditory moving window presentation. journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(4):P298-311.,5656 Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95.,5757 Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn H-J, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23(8):1317-26.,6767 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93.

WM has an impact on several language domain, such as narrative discourse,4949 Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. 2005;19:1001-9. completeness and story grammar,3434 Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48. conversational discourse,4242 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Early conversational discourse abilities following traumatic brain injury: An acute predictive study. Brain Inj. 2014;28(7): 951-8. inference production,4646 Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46. sentence repetition,5252 Small J a, Kemper S, Lyons K. Sentence repetition and processing resources in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2000;75:232-58. number of correct answers in a writing task,5757 Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn H-J, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23(8):1317-26. improvement in naming and comprehension after rehabilitation6060 Seniów J, Litwin M, Lesniak M. The relationship between non-linguistic cognitive deficits and language recovery in patients with aphasia. J Neurol Sci. 2009;283(1-2):91-4. and grammatical comprehension.6161 Grossman M, Moore P. A longitudinal study of sentence comprehension difficulty in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76(5):644-9. However, no relationship was found between WM measures and pauses produced between clauses4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285. or in online syntactic processing.5353 Waters G, Caplan D. Working memory and online syntactic processing in Alzheimer's disease: studies with auditory moving window presentation. journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(4):P298-311.

Other cognitive tests used in several articles included the Trail Making Test,3636 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93.,4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.,4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.,4848 Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9.,5050 Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: An exploratory study using the GALI (a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.,5656 Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95. the Wisconsin Test3131 Fridriksson J, Nettles C, Davis M, Morrow L, Montgomery A. Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clin Linguist Phon. 2006;20(6):401-10.,5050 Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: An exploratory study using the GALI (a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.,4646 Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46.,6868 Knibb JA, Woollams AM, Hodges JR, Patterson K. Making sense of progressive non-fluent aphasia: An analysis of conversational speech. Brain. 2009;132(10):2734-46.) and the Stroop test.4848 Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9.,5050 Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: An exploratory study using the GALI (a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.,4545 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.,5454 Feyereisen P, Berrewaerts J, Hupet M. Pragmatic skills in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease: An analysis by means of a referential communication task. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42:1-17.,5757 Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn H-J, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23(8):1317-26. The Trail Making Test is a valuable tool for assessing cognitive flexibility, working memory, set-shifting and inhibitory abilities.6969 Müller LD, Guhn A, Zeller JBM, Biehl SC, Dresler T, Hahn T, et al. Neural correlates of a standardized version of the trail making test in young and elderly adults: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56(1):271-9. The word-color Stroop test, however, measures attention, cognitive flexibility, inhibition and information processing speed.7070 Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop Color-Word Interference Test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. Arango-Lasprilla JC, editor. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):591-624. The Wisconsin Test is a widely used neuropsychological task for the assessment of higher-order cognitive functioning, and test performance is associated with abstract reasoning, strategic planning, organized searching, and impulse control abilities.7070 Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop Color-Word Interference Test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. Arango-Lasprilla JC, editor. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):591-624. However, it has been heavily criticized for being too general a tool for EF measurement.

Interestingly, one of the most used paradigms for both language and other neuropsychological domain assessments was verbal fluency (VF), in both its modalities, categorical and phonemic. It was present in studies of RBD,3939 Zimmermann N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal Fluency in Right Brain Damage: Dissociations Among Production Criteria and Duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8. dementia5151 Herbert V, Brookes RL, Markus HS, Morris RG. Verbal Fluency in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease and Alzheimer's Disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(4):413-21,5555 Stopford CL, Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D. Distinct Memory Profiles in Alzheimer's Disease. Cortex. 2007;43(7):846-57.

56 Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95.
-5757 Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn H-J, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23(8):1317-26.,7171 Bastin C, Yakushev I, Bahri MA, Fellgiebel A, Eustache F, Landeau B, et al. Cognitive reserve impacts on inter-individual variability in resting-state cerebral metabolism in normal aging. Neuroimage. 2012;63(2):713-22. and TBI4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.

44 Douglas JM. Relation of Executive Functioning to Pragmatic Outcome Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365.

45 Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.
-4646 Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46.,4848 Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9.,6767 LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93. results were compared to findings on tests of memory, mental flexibility, confrontation naming, semantic and letter category naming, verbal reasoning, and to scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. The relationship to outcome as measured with the Disability Rating Scale (DRS. VF tasks are often used in clinical and research practice, and their wide-spread use is partially due to their validity for both verbal and executive abilities. It is considered an effective screening instrument for general verbal functioning and its validity for assessing executive control is well established.7272 Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014 Jul 22;5(JUL):1-10.

Cognitive assessment can be especially challenging in patients suffering from linguistic deficits given they present problems in comprehension. Those problems may result in misleading scores on cognitive tests, because when patients are unable to understand what they have been asked to do, their performance will be affected.6262 Potagas C, Kasselimis D, Evdokimidis I. Short-term and working memory impairments in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2874-8. Therefore, it is important to devise cognitive screening tests and batteries based on the main executive functions and in the most used neuropsychological tests specifically designed for language impairment.

This kind of battery could be useful for speech language therapeutic use, since it would be a rapid way of identifying the EF deficits that can impact communication. On the other hand, it is also necessary to draw on the further understanding of the relationship between language and EF to devise tests and evidence-based interventions designed for speech language therapists. Furthermore, this could also raise awareness of neurologists and neuropsychologists regarding the importance of assessing communication disorders when assessing cognitive functions.

Several EF such as planning, working memory, accurate self-monitoring, and judgment are necessary skills for patients to engage in, and benefit from speech language therapy.7373 Votruba KL, Rapport LJ, Whitman RD, Johnson A, Langenecker S. Personality Differences among Patients with Chronic Aphasia Predict Improvement in Speech-Language Therapy. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2013;20(5):421-31. They play an important role for rehabilitation because they underpin several complex behaviors needed for functional independence and social integration.7474 Henry JD, Crawford JR. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(4):621-8. The understanding of the relationship between EF and language could help design optimal interventions for patients suffering with language disorders. Therefore, an important step for developing communication strategies by health care professionals, caregivers and patients themselves is to try to identify the nature of the problems that are causing communication breakdowns.

As shown throughout this article, language and EF have a complex relationship, therefore a complete assessment of patients with an acquired language disorder should reflect the dynamic processing of cognitive brain functions, as opposed to a compartmentalized one. Therefore, it is essential that clinicians, language speech therapists and neuropsychologists work as a team to offer the best healthcare service possible.

Some limitations of this study relate to the fact that the articles selected have very heterogeneous samples when compared, which can have an impact on the results. Also, many different types of linguistic processes were included, therefore the relationship of EF with which aspect of language processing was unclear. Another limitation concerns the use of only one research engine for the articles search.

EF appears to play a crucial role in comprehension and production of linguistic macrostructures3333 Chabok SY, Kapourchali SR, Leili EK, Saberi A, Mohtasham-Amiri Z. Effective factors on linguistic disorder during acute phase following traumatic brain injury in adults. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(7):1444-50. and microstructures.4343 Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285. However, the extent to which EF interferes with several language domains is still unclear and needs to be further elucidated. Of the main benefits of considering the relationship of language and EF subprocess, we highlight the accuracy of rehabilitation planning. During rehabilitation, for example, clinicians could use this relationship to improve both processes. Some linguistic symptoms are strongly related to a dysexecutive syndrome, such as perseveration, off-target verbosity, stereotype, and could benefit from this vision.

More studies should be conducted to better understand the relationship between language and EF and its influence on speech language therapy. Further, future studies should explore the influence of other components that could interfere in this relationship. It should be considered that executive functioning it not only responsible for basic cognitive processes, but also for complex systems that integrate these abilities.7474 Henry JD, Crawford JR. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(4):621-8.

  • This study was conducted at the Psychology Department of the Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Verma M, Howard RJ. Semantic memory and language dysfunction in early Alzheimer's disease: a review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27(12):1209-17.
  • 2
    Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, Cappa SF, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76(11):1006-14.
  • 3
    Constantinidou F, Wertheimer JC, Tsanadis J, Evans C, Paul DR. Assessment of executive functioning in brain injury: Collaboration between speech-language pathology and neuropsychology for an integrative neuropsychological perspective. Brain Inj. 2012;26:13-4.
  • 4
    Koehler C, Gindri G, Mancopes R. demência avaliados com a Bateria MAC Language alterations in elderly patients with dementia assessed with the MAC Battery. 2012;17(1):15-22.
  • 5
    Jamadar S, Powers NR, Meda SA, Calhoun VD, Gelernter J, Gruen JR, et al. Genetic influences of resting state fMRI activity in language-related brain regions in healthy controls and schizophrenia patients: A pilot study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2013;7(1):15-27.
  • 6
    Togher L, McDonald S, Tate R, Rietdijk R, Power E. The effectiveness of social communication partner training for adults with severe chronic TBI and their families using a measure of perceived communication ability. Yeates G, editor. NeuroRehabilitation. 2016;38(3):243-55.
  • 7
    El Hachioui H, Visch-Brink EG, de Lau LML, van de Sandt-Koenderman MWME, Nouwens F, Koudstaal PJ, et al. Screening tests for aphasia in patients with stroke: a systematic review. J Neurol. 2017;264(2):211-20.
  • 8
    Ferris SH, Farlow M. Language impairment in alzheimer's disease and benefits of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8:1007-14.
  • 9
    Arrighi HM, Neumann PJ, Lieberburg IM, Townsend RJ. Lethality of Alzheimer disease and its impact on nursing home placement. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(1):90-5.
  • 10
    Alzheimer Association. 2013 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimer's Dement. 2013;9(2):208-45.
  • 11
    Szatloczki G, Hoffmann I, Vincze V, Kalman J, Pakaski M. Speaking in Alzheimer's disease, is that an early sign? Importance of changes in language abilities in Alzheimer's disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:1-7.
  • 12
    Appell J, Kertesz A, Fisman M. A study of language functioning in Alzheimer patients. Brain Lang. 1982;17(1):73-91.
  • 13
    Croot K, Hodges JR, Xuereb J, Patterson K. Phonological and articulatory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: A case series. Brain Lang. 2000;75(2):277-309.
  • 14
    Kempler D. Neurocognitive Disorders in Aging. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2004.
  • 15
    Damasio AR. Aphasia. N Engl J Med. 1992 Feb 20;326(8):531-9.
  • 16
    Davidson B, Howe T, Worrall L, Hickson L, Togher L. Social Participation for Older People with Aphasia: The Impact of Communication Disability on Friendships. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2008;15(4):325-40.
  • 17
    Stahl B, Van Lancker Sidtis D. Tapping into neural resources of communication: formulaic language in aphasia therapy. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1526.
  • 18
    Johns CL, Tooley KM, Traxler MJ. Discourse Impairments Following Right Hemisphere Brain Damage: A Critical Review. Lang Linguist Compass. 2008;(6):1038-62.
  • 19
    Myers P. Right Hemisphere Damage: Disorders of Communication and Cognition. Norwich: Singular; 1999.
  • 20
    Lundgren K, Brownell H. Figurative Language Deficits Associated With Right Hemisphere Disorder. Perspect ASHA Spec Interes Groups. 2016;1(2):66.
  • 21
    Coelho CA. Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Semin Speech Lang. 2007;28(2):122-35.
  • 22
    Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short term memory, working memory,and syntactic comprehension in Aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109.
  • 23
    Mayer JF, Murray LL. Measuring working memory deficits in aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2012;45(5):325-39.
  • 24
    Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex "Frontal Lobe" Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100.
  • 25
    Collins A, Koechlin E. Reasoning, Learning, and Creativity: Frontal Lobe Function and Human Decision-Making. PLoS Biol. 2012;10(3):e1001293.
  • 26
    Lehto JE, Juujärvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br J Dev Psychol. 2003;21(1):59-80.
  • 27
    Lunt L, Bramham J, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Selway RP, Xenitidis K, et al. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and "Jumping to Conclusions": Bias or deficit? J Neuropsychol. 2012;6(1):65-78.
  • 28
    Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex "Frontal Lobe" Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100.
  • 29
    Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-68.
  • 30
    McDonald S, Togher L, Code C. Social and Communication Disorders Following Traumatic Brain Injury. 2nd ed. McDonald S, Togher L, Code C, editors. Social Communication Disorders Following Traumatic Brain Injury. New York: Routledge; 2013.
  • 31
    Fridriksson J, Nettles C, Davis M, Morrow L, Montgomery A. Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clin Linguist Phon. 2006;20(6):401-10.
  • 32
    Zimmermann N, Gindri G, Oliveira CR de, Fonseca RP. Pragmatic and Executive Functions in Traumatic Brain Injury and Right Brain Damage. Dement Neuropsychol. 2011;5(4):337-45.
  • 33
    Chabok SY, Kapourchali SR, Leili EK, Saberi A, Mohtasham-Amiri Z. Effective factors on linguistic disorder during acute phase following traumatic brain injury in adults. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(7):1444-50.
  • 34
    Coelho C, Lê K, Mozeiko J, Hamilton M, Tyler E, Krueger F, et al. Characterizing discourse deficits following penetrating head injury: A preliminary model. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S438-48.
  • 35
    Turkstra LS. Conversation-based assessment of social cognition in adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2008;22(5):397-409.
  • 36
    LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93.
  • 37
    Helm-Estabrooks N. Cognition and aphasia: a discussion and a study. J Commun Disord. 2002;35(2):171-86.
  • 38
    McDonald S. Exploring the cognitive basis of right-hemisphere pragmatic language disorders. Brain Lang. 2000;75(1):82-107.
  • 39
    Zimmermann N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal Fluency in Right Brain Damage: Dissociations Among Production Criteria and Duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8.
  • 40
    Birn RM, Kenworthy L, Case L, Caravella R, Jones TB, Bandettini PA, et al. of Verbal Fluency. 2011;49(1):1099-107.
  • 41
    Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19(12):1001-9.
  • 42
    LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Early conversational discourse abilities following traumatic brain injury: An acute predictive study. Brain Inj. 2014;28(7): 951-8.
  • 43
    Peach RK. The Cognitive Basis for Sentence Planning Difficulties in Discourse After Traumatic Brain Injury. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2013;22(2):S285.
  • 44
    Douglas JM. Relation of Executive Functioning to Pragmatic Outcome Following Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365.
  • 45
    Thomas-Antérion C, Borg C, Teissèdre M, Sciessere K, Extier C, Bedoin N. A study of semantic memory after brain injury: Learning newly coined French words. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2009;52(9):610-22.
  • 46
    Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Bales JW. Understanding text after severe closed-head injury: Assessing inferences and memory operations with a think-aloud procedure. Brain Lang. 2005;94(3):331-46.
  • 47
    Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury?: An exploratory study using the GALI ( a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange ) §. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.
  • 48
    Rousseaux M, Vérigneaux C, Kozlowski O. An analysis of communication in conversation after severe traumatic brain injury. Eur J Neurol. 2010;17(7):922-9.
  • 49
    Youse KM, Coelho CA. Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. 2005;19:1001-9.
  • 50
    Sainson C, Barat M, Aguert M. Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: An exploratory study using the GALI (a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2014;57(9-10):664-83.
  • 51
    Herbert V, Brookes RL, Markus HS, Morris RG. Verbal Fluency in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease and Alzheimer's Disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(4):413-21
  • 52
    Small J a, Kemper S, Lyons K. Sentence repetition and processing resources in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2000;75:232-58.
  • 53
    Waters G, Caplan D. Working memory and online syntactic processing in Alzheimer's disease: studies with auditory moving window presentation. journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(4):P298-311.
  • 54
    Feyereisen P, Berrewaerts J, Hupet M. Pragmatic skills in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease: An analysis by means of a referential communication task. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2007;42:1-17.
  • 55
    Stopford CL, Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D. Distinct Memory Profiles in Alzheimer's Disease. Cortex. 2007;43(7):846-57.
  • 56
    Wardlow L, Ivanova I, Gollan TH. Neuropsychologia The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners?: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer ' s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56:184-95.
  • 57
    Yoon JH, Suh MK, Jeong Y, Ahn H-J, Moon SY, Chin J, et al. Agraphia in Korean patients with early onset Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23(8):1317-26.
  • 58
    Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short-term memory, working memory, and syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013;30(2):77-109.
  • 59
    Ivanova M V., Hallowell B. A new modified listening span task to enhance validity of working memory assessment for people with and without aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2014;52:78-98.
  • 60
    Seniów J, Litwin M, Lesniak M. The relationship between non-linguistic cognitive deficits and language recovery in patients with aphasia. J Neurol Sci. 2009;283(1-2):91-4.
  • 61
    Grossman M, Moore P. A longitudinal study of sentence comprehension difficulty in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76(5):644-9.
  • 62
    Potagas C, Kasselimis D, Evdokimidis I. Short-term and working memory impairments in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2874-8.
  • 63
    Murray LL. Attention and other cognitive deficits in aphasia: Presence and relation to language and communication measures. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2012;21(2):51-65.
  • 64
    Knibb JA, Woollams AM, Hodges JR, Patterson K. Making sense of progressive non-fluent aphasia?: an analysis of conversational speech. 2009;2734-46.
  • 65
    Hebert-Myers H, Guttentag CL, Swank PR, Smith KE, Landry SH. The Importance of Language, Social, and Behavioral Skills Across Early and Later Childhood as Predictors of Social Competence With Peers. Appl Dev Sci. 2006;10(4):174-87.
  • 66
    Marini A, Galetto V, Zampieri E, Vorano L, Zettin M, Carlomagno S. Narrative language in traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2904-10.
  • 67
    LeBlanc J, de Guise E, Champoux M-C, Couturier C, Lamoureux J, Marcoux J, et al. Acute evaluation of conversational discourse skills in traumatic brain injury. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014;16(6):582-93.
  • 68
    Knibb JA, Woollams AM, Hodges JR, Patterson K. Making sense of progressive non-fluent aphasia: An analysis of conversational speech. Brain. 2009;132(10):2734-46.
  • 69
    Müller LD, Guhn A, Zeller JBM, Biehl SC, Dresler T, Hahn T, et al. Neural correlates of a standardized version of the trail making test in young and elderly adults: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuropsychologia. 2014;56(1):271-9.
  • 70
    Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop Color-Word Interference Test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. Arango-Lasprilla JC, editor. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):591-624.
  • 71
    Bastin C, Yakushev I, Bahri MA, Fellgiebel A, Eustache F, Landeau B, et al. Cognitive reserve impacts on inter-individual variability in resting-state cerebral metabolism in normal aging. Neuroimage. 2012;63(2):713-22.
  • 72
    Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014 Jul 22;5(JUL):1-10.
  • 73
    Votruba KL, Rapport LJ, Whitman RD, Johnson A, Langenecker S. Personality Differences among Patients with Chronic Aphasia Predict Improvement in Speech-Language Therapy. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2013;20(5):421-31.
  • 74
    Henry JD, Crawford JR. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(4):621-8.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jul-Sep 2018

History

  • Received
    19 Feb 2018
  • Accepted
    18 June 2018
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br