Abstracts
Specific neuropsychiatric disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) affect some forms of memory while leaving others relatively intact. In this review, we investigate particularities of the relationship between explicit and implicit memories in AD. It was found that implicit memory is preserved in AD, irrespective of the task used; in other words, there was not interference from explicit memory. In addition, it was verified that is possible through implicit memory compensatory strategies such as, activities of daily living (ADL) to compensate for the explicit memory deficits. In this sense, cognitive rehabilitation (CR) demonstrates reasonable results in the process of compensation of explicit memory deficits. Concluding, the decline in explicit memory suggests that both systems are functionally independent even if the other is compromised. We expect that when explicit memory system is not involved in competition with the implicit system, the final effect of learning is better, because all of the implicit memory capacity is engaged in learning and not in competition with the explicit system.
activities of daily living; Alzheimer's disease; cognitive rehabilitation; implicit memory
Distúrbios neuropsiquiátricos específicos, tais como a doença de Alzheimer (DA), podem afetar algumas formas de memória enquanto deixam outros relativamente intactos. Nesta revisão, nós investigamos particularidades da relação entre as memórias explicita e implícita na DA. Foi verificado que a memória é preservada na DA, independente da tarefa usada; ou seja, não ocorre interferência da memória explícita. Além disso, foi verificado que é possível através de estratégias compensatórias de memória implícita, tais como, atividades da vida diária (AVD) compensar os déficits da memória explícita. Neste sentido, a reabilitação cognitiva (RC) demonstra resultados razoáveis no processo de compensação dos déficits da memória explicita. Concluindo, a queda na memória explícita sugere que ambos os sistemas são funcionalmente independentes mesmo que outro esteja comprometido. Esperamos que quando o sistema de memória explícita não está envolvido em competição com o sistema implícito, o efeito final de aprendizagem é melhor, porque toda a capacidade da memória implícita está engajada na aprendizagem e não na competição com o sistema explícito.
atividades da vida diária; doença de Alzheimer; memória implícita; reabilitação cognitiva
VIEWS AND REVIEWS
Alzheimer's disease and implicit memory
Doença de Alzheimer e memória implícita
Sergio MachadoI,III; Marlo CunhaI,III; Daniel MincI; Claudio Elidio PortellaI; Bruna VelasquesI,III; Luis F. BasileIV,V; Maurício CagyVI; Roberto PiedadeI; Pedro RibeiroI,II,III
IBrain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (IPUB/UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil
IISchool of Physical Education, Bioscience Department (EEFD/UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil
IIIBrazilian Institute of Neural Bioscience (IBBN), Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil
IVDivision of Neurosurgery, University of São Paulo Medical School, Brazil
VLaboratory of Psychophysiology, Faculdade de Psicologia e Fonoaudiologia, UMESP, Brazil
VIDivision of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Institute of Health Community, Federal Fluminense University (UFF), Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil
ABSTRACT
Specific neuropsychiatric disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) affect some forms of memory while leaving others relatively intact. In this review, we investigate particularities of the relationship between explicit and implicit memories in AD. It was found that implicit memory is preserved in AD, irrespective of the task used; in other words, there was not interference from explicit memory. In addition, it was verified that is possible through implicit memory compensatory strategies such as, activities of daily living (ADL) to compensate for the explicit memory deficits. In this sense, cognitive rehabilitation (CR) demonstrates reasonable results in the process of compensation of explicit memory deficits. Concluding, the decline in explicit memory suggests that both systems are functionally independent even if the other is compromised. We expect that when explicit memory system is not involved in competition with the implicit system, the final effect of learning is better, because all of the implicit memory capacity is engaged in learning and not in competition with the explicit system.
Key words: activities of daily living, Alzheimer's disease, cognitive rehabilitation, implicit memory.
RESUMO
Distúrbios neuropsiquiátricos específicos, tais como a doença de Alzheimer (DA), podem afetar algumas formas de memória enquanto deixam outros relativamente intactos. Nesta revisão, nós investigamos particularidades da relação entre as memórias explicita e implícita na DA. Foi verificado que a memória é preservada na DA, independente da tarefa usada; ou seja, não ocorre interferência da memória explícita. Além disso, foi verificado que é possível através de estratégias compensatórias de memória implícita, tais como, atividades da vida diária (AVD) compensar os déficits da memória explícita. Neste sentido, a reabilitação cognitiva (RC) demonstra resultados razoáveis no processo de compensação dos déficits da memória explicita. Concluindo, a queda na memória explícita sugere que ambos os sistemas são funcionalmente independentes mesmo que outro esteja comprometido. Esperamos que quando o sistema de memória explícita não está envolvido em competição com o sistema implícito, o efeito final de aprendizagem é melhor, porque toda a capacidade da memória implícita está engajada na aprendizagem e não na competição com o sistema explícito.
Palavras-chave: atividades da vida diária, doença de Alzheimer, memória implícita, reabilitação cognitiva.
Memory is the recording, retention and retrieval of information. It accounts for all knowledge gained through experience. Memory is not a unitary process, but is composed of dissociable systems that mediate specific types of mnemonic function1. Specific neuropsychiatric disorders, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) can affect some forms of memory while leaving others relatively intact. One form of memory, explicit memory (EM)2,3, is the ability to consciously and directly recall or recognize recently processed information. It is under the control of the hippocampus and temporal lobe connections, essentials for the formation of new episodic memories4,5. This type of memory is highly impaired in AD patients and is usually the first symptom of dementia. It has been linked to pathological, structural and functional abnormalities within the mesial temporal lobe (MTL) and diencephalic structures. Such facts are based on studies using post-mortem examination6, structural imaging7, resting metabolism8 and functional imaging9-14.
Another form of memory, implicit memory (IM), is the ability to improve task performance. It reflects the unconscious effects of previous experiences on subsequent task performance, without conscious recollection. IM is independent of the middle temporal lobe (MTL) and diencephalic structures14. It is involved with the unconscious recognition of an object (i.e., priming) and the correct completion of the steps in a task (i.e., procedural memory)15. IM is assessed indirectly by measuring facilitation in performance (i.e. decreased processing time or increased accuracy) due to previous exposure to identical or related information. It has been consistently shown that procedural memory remains relatively preserved throughout the course of AD16-21.
In this review, our objective was investigating particularities of the relationship between explicit and implicit memories in AD. In this sense, we focus on the following questions: (a) if implicit memory is affected by explicit memory deficits; (b) if implicit memory compensatory strategies such as activity of daily living (ADL) and could be employed to compensate for the explicit memory deficits. According to the above questions, we developed a strategy for searching studies in the main databases. The computer-supported search used the following databases: Pubmed, Medline, ISI Web of Knowledge and Scielo. Only clinical and experimental reports published in English and conducted from 1987 up to 2008 were considered. The key words used were: implicit memory, habit learning, motor learning, motor skills, priming, procedural memory and sequence learning, all of them in combination with Alzheimer's disease. The inclusion criteria for the studies related to first question were: (a) a clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease based on specified and generally accepted criteria; (b) implicit tasks; (c) task performance expressed in time or error measures, and not only in fMRI or other imaging data.
ALZHEIMER DISEASE AS A MODEL TO UNDERSTAND MEMORY
Models may be literally viewed as a partial approximation of reality, in the sense that we do not intend to replace the phenomenon per se, but compose them by elements of the phenomenon22. By this definition, models are parts of the reality that researchers have chosen to assemble together in order to more clearly observe the phenomenon itself23. Pathologies that impair the central nervous system (CNS) functions lead to pathophysiological states that can be seen as models for our understanding of how the healthy CNS works. Scientists have also been strenuously trying to figure out different patterns of pathological behavior by means of models24. At the same time, CNS diseases being a synonym of pain and suffering for patients and family members, the use of models that select specific disease peculiarities may aid us in the prevention, better management during the course of the disease, and occasionally its cure. AD is no exception to this paradigm. AD has become a "stage" type of model for the study of mental processes, in particular of different kinds of memory. Over the last 30 years, an increasing amount of time, effort and money have been devoted to decipher memory storage and recovery processes by means of the AD pathophysiological model. During this era, a vast amount of physiological knowledge was produced in our attempts to cure or slow down the normal course of AD. The present manuscript is a contribution to this effort, aiming at the relationship between explicit and implicit memory and their possible mechanisms, where we review state of the art experiments designed to uncover mechanisms of explicit and implicit memory by their relations with the AD process.
EXPLICIT MEMORY DEFICITS AFFECT THE IMPLICIT MEMORY?
A task performance is a complex network of operations. A subject may be instructed to remember a previous event (i.e., explicit retrieval task) or simply perform a task with no reference to a previous event (i.e., implicit retrieval task). However, one may evoke any cognitive process in performing a memory task, and thus may intentionally evoke explicit processing (e.g., using previous memory) to improve performance on an implicit task. This is relating to as explicit contamination4,25,26. Several older individuals, with and without AD, have explicit memory impairment, therefore, using implicit tasks that are known to draw some degree on explicit processing will result in compromised implicit memory compared to young subjects. To address this problem, it is important to use priming tasks that are known in AD patients27. Other methods that have been used to measure and to minimize explicit contamination in priming include using within-task manipulations, such as depth-of-encoding, that affect explicit but not implicit performance28, presenting test-phase stimuli very briefly25,29, and extending the interval between the study and the test phase29. In addition, Mitchell and Brus29 verified that priming was constant when different groups (i.e., young, middle aged and old subjects) were intentionally submitted to explicit strategies in implicit retrieval. Moreover, the authors concluded that most implicit memory processes remain stable and justifiably cautioned that explicit contamination of implicit retrieval should be rigorously monitored in aging studies.
According to evidence, priming and procedural memory share a few underlying neural activation in the neostriatal system, where gradual learning occurs across trials, even without conscious recognition. However, each process has separate mechanisms26,30,31. Priming is influenced by frontal cortex, which may reflect its attentional demands32-34 and procedural memory is influenced by the striatum, sensorimotor cortex and cerebellum30,35,36, which may reproduce its motor skills. Explicit memory impairment is one of the earliest AD symptoms, and its loss characterizes all the stages of AD37. Although some types of implicit memory are also lost in AD, certainly, other ones are preserved into late stages of disease5. The neurodegenerative pattern of AD progression and the dissociation of implicit and explicit memory enable patients to preserve some types of implicit memory despite of severe explicit loss. This finding is supported by evidence from priming and procedural memory studies, demonstrating that individuals in the mild to moderate stages of AD show improvement in implicit memory tasks5,38,39. In this sense, interventions in AD patients have potential to maintain function through increased speed or accuracy when completing a task.
When these patients are repeatedly expose to an object, these patients can process information faster and/or more accurately compared to baseline5,40. The perceptual priming spare in AD patients has been demonstrated using words38, objects nonverbal responses to pictorial material41,42 and a picture fragment task38,43. On the contrary, conceptual priming becomes impaired early in AD42,44,45 due to its reliance on semantic memory, which is also impaired in early stages of AD. Thus, AD patients demonstrate intact perceptual priming despite impaired conceptual priming5,43,46. Moreover, long-term retention of procedural memory among mild- to moderate-stage AD patients has been found when the tasks are practiced under constant but not in random practice conditions21. These patients demonstrate the same amount of improvement in implicit skill learning (despite a lower baseline performance) on tasks as normal control subjects21,47. Performance improvement in these implicit memory skills among AD patients is similar to healthy controls5. According to such facts, these dissociations account for a common clinical situation: AD patients who can accurately use a toothbrush while they can no longer name a picture of a toothbrush or describe the steps used. In this sense, in the next two subsections we will discuss whether implicit memory is affected by explicit memory deficits.
Modulation of implicit learning capacity due to explicit memory deficits in ad patients
Several studies highlight the implicit learning capacity of AD patients. Some studies using a Maze test in which blindfolded subjects had to trace a complex pathway reporting that the AD patients were able to learn new motor-skills implicitly48-51. Other studies apply a Rotor-Pursuit task, where subjects had to maintain contact between a hand-held stylus and a rotating spot, demonstrating preserved learning abilities in AD patients52-56. In agreement with the above findings, studies based on a Puzzle-Assembly task57 and based on a Mirror-Tracing task47 shown the same results' pattern. In order to investigate this capacity in AD patients, others experiments used the Serial Reaction-Time task (SRTT) in which subjects had to respond as fast as possible when a stimulus appeared in one of four places by pressing a response key. It was observed that AD patients demonstrated implicit learning capacity as reflected by the difference in reaction times (RTs) between blocks with a decrease in RTs in a fixed sequence of stimuli presentation and prolonged RTs in a random block56,58-60.
On the contrary, others studies, indicate that the implicit learning capacity in AD patients is affected because they presented inferior outcomes when accuracy was taken into account56 or when the data were log-transformed because of the unequal variance in RT60. However, Ferraro's experiment found preserved procedural memory (SRTT) only in the "very mildly AD" group and less in the "mildly AD" group although it is relevant to mention that almost all studies used a subtle disease classification61. Thus, regardless of task used, the studies assessing Procedural Memory in AD patients and shown positive outcomes. Indeed, Hirono et al. observed that patients with mild AD were able to acquire motor and perceptual as well as cognitive skills in various motor skills learning tasks62.
Remaining performance level and amount of learning in AD patients
According to the previous discussion about the preserved implicit learning capacities in AD patients, our next question is for how long this process will last? In this manner, two aspects in implicit memory issue should be differentiated, i.e., overall performance level and amount of learning, for example, the decrease in RT in the SRTT task. Several studies found preserved procedural memory in AD patients, although their performance level in terms of reaction and movement time were inferior when compared with controls. However, when it takes the level of learning into account, the results are less consistent.
Some results were not reported with enough detail to show unambiguously the amount of learning which AD patients shown when compared to the controls57. Some comparative studies did not include a healthy control group in addition to the patient groups50-52,59,63 preventing patient-control comparisons. Other studies using a Maze test reported learning capacities in the AD group but less improvement across trials compared to the controls findings48,49, which can be explained by the use of a task without visual feedback. In the same way, in SRTT studies AD patients shown the same amount of learning (decrease in RT during the blocks with the fixed sequence) as the normal controls56,58,60,61. The Rotor-Pursuit experiments there were also no patient-control differences in amount of learning53-56,58,62,64-66. Taken together, all studies shown preserved procedural memory in AD patients regardless of the task used.
Their performance levels, however, never reached the levels of the healthy controls, demonstrated by their prolonged reaction and movement times. The AD patients' level of learning also varied depending on the task to be performed. It suggests that visual feedback has a positive effect on their learning pace. They also seem to experience more problems with motor skills learning when performing the SRTT in two learning processes. Therefore, the subjects have to dominate both spatial and motor regularities67, do not remembering that is the learning of spatial regularities that may be impaired in AD patients, a process that is less implicated in the Rotor-Pursuit task in normal motor skills learning in AD patients.
IMPLICIT MEMORY MAY BE USED A COMPENSATORY STRATEGY TO COMPENSATE FOR THE EXPLICIT MEMORY DEFICITS?
During the initial stages of explicit learning, involving trial and error, subjects have to find out the correct movement. The critical requirement is the novel establishment of perceived sensory cues with the correct motor commands. For this purpose, subjects have to attend to sensory cues. They have to decide which movement will be initiated immediately (if feedback is given) and they have to encode the perceived response in memory. Thus, the establishment of a novel arbitrary sensorimotor association (as it is required during learning by trial and error) is closely related to attention68, decision and selection of movements, sensory feedback processing and working memory. Once subjects figure out the correct movements the map of sensorimotor translation is provided. Sensory stimuli have to be retained in working memory to be translated to the motor output69, performance of actions is still slow and unskilled and feedback and attentional processing play a critical role70.
In this manner, during the implicit learning (i.e., with practice), sensorimotor maps become stronger and are stored in long-term memory. Visual cues are transformed accurately and rapidly to the precise motor response. Hence, action can be performed with less intensive sensory feedback processing and at higher speed. After long-term practice, movements become automatic and can be performed at high speed and accuracy, even if subjects do not attend to the action. Thus, variables such as practice and feedback can be structured differently to enhance learning at each stage. Feedback in early stage, for example, may need to be more specific and applied more frequently to enhance learning, while feedback may be weaned toward the third stage of learning71. It has been proposed that in implicit learning the three stages might overlap or be ordered differently. There is support for a parallel development of implicit and explicit knowledge in learning72.
According to the evidence found in the previous section, AD patients seem to have the implicit memory preserved. Thus, in the following section, we will discuss if through implicit memory compensatory strategies, such as: ADL is possible to compensate for the explicit memory deficits. In this manner, we based on two variables, practice and feedback, that play a worthwhile role in implicit memory.
Practice role in AD patients' performance
In relation to variable practice, the principle "the more you practice, the more you learn", implies that the amount of practice should maximize the implicit memory. However, does more practice improve the implicit memory performance in AD patients? Dick et al.54 found on the Rotor Pursuit that both AD and control groups had reached their optimal performance after 40 trials due to subsequent practice failed to yield any additional augmenting effect. It would be interesting to determine whether this also holds for other tasks like the Maze test in which, relative to the controls, an inferior amount of learning was observed for AD patients48,49. In this sense, some issues still create doubts. An important factor in relation to the types of practice is what is the best type of practice to preserve implicit memory? Other one that merits closer attention is whether the task should be learned as a whole or per components. Moreover, the amount of practice is also an important question that merits a greater investigation. In relation to this discuss, there are three traditional terms in the literature, random, blocked and constant. Early evidence suggests that random practice might be most effective to acquire motor skills, whereas during the acquisition of a specific motor-skill, performance benefits most from blocked practice73.
All available studies show that AD patients learn best under constant practice conditions18-21. According to Dick et al.18, humans use their episodic memory of the training trials to accurately perform a task while learning a skill. They suggest that because AD patients experience problems with episodic memory, constant practice is more effective due to repeated running of the same neural networks (NNs) and does not require an intact episodic memory. The second reason why random practice may be less effective is that other cognitive functions that play a role in random practice, e.g., the ability to divide attention, are affected in AD patients.
In this manner, Dick et al.18,21 try to explain the superior learning performance of AD patients under constant practice condition in terms of the schema theory originally developed by Schmidt74. In this experiment, the authors propose a more open-loop account for motor control. The theory of Schmidt assumes the existence of generalized NNs that are acquired through practice and that define the "form" of the action. Moreover, Schmidt claims that these NNs can be altered to meet environmental demands by a closed-loop system using sensory feedback75. Consequently, Dick et al.18,21 based on the theory of Schmidt concluded that AD patients can develop and access a NN in training situations that emphasize movement consistency. However, they do not formed the NNs needed to successfully achieve a movement when the environmental demands change because they are unable to encode and to store the different types of information about a motor pattern.
Feedback role in AD patients' performance
Other crucial variable that influences implicit learning is the type of feedback. There are two types of feedback, intrinsic where the sensory information is provided by motion and extrinsic where information comes from an external source like a verbal command. These types of feedback can contain summary or constant information of performance. It is known that constant feedback enhances only motor performance, not the level of learning. With less frequent feedback, learners have to depend on other cues, which entail more elaborate encoding. In this sense, extrinsic feedback can be shared into "knowledge of results", in which the movement outcome is given in terms of goal, and "knowledge of performance", where the feedback provides the movement pattern itself, e.g., a decreased response time in a SRTT task73.
When observed, almost all studies on motor-skill learning in AD patients employed visual feedback. However, only the Maze tasks were applied according to blindfolded conditions and the amount of learning in the AD patients proved inferior when compared with the controls48,49. In another way, the Rotor-Pursuit experiments individualize the velocity of the target to equate initial performance. According to these findings, some studies indicate that controls generally tracked at a faster rate than the AD patients54,55,63,64. Possibly, AD patients can only perform this task at a slower rate because they depend on visual feedback more than controls. The only experiment using a Rotor-Pursuit task in explicit form investigated the role of visual feedback on performance in AD patients. It was demonstrated a decrease in performance when the visibility of the moving target was reduced during the learning phase53. On the contrary of the normal controls, the performance of patients did not improve across trials in the restricted-vision condition. In the full-vision condition the patients shown normal learning. It appears that constant visual feedback is important in implicit learning for AD patients. However, it was not found any studies concerning the frequency of external feedback, and whether knowledge of results and knowledge of performance makes a difference in AD group.
In this manner, our results suggest that both forms of feedback knowledge probably place too much weight on the cognitive abilities in AD patients and therefore contribute little to successful performance. According to previous results, the recent findings of Tippett and Sergio76 demonstrated that the integration of eye and hand information may be impaired in AD patients. In this experiment, it was investigated whether the accuracy of movements requiring a visuomotor transformation in neurologically healthy elderly subjects compared with patients diagnosed with probable Alzheimer's disease. Subjects made sliding finger movements over a clear touch-sensitive screen positioned in three spatial planes to displace a cursor from a central target to one of four peripheral targets viewed on a monitor. These spatial plane were repeated under conditions where the direction of cursor motion was rotated 180º relative to the direction of hand motion. Significant results were observed between AD patients and control groups on reaction time and movement time measures. Also, significant increases in task completion errors were observed in AD population. Further, performance was more affected by visual feedback changes relative to the plane location changes. Notably, there were substantial deficits observed in AD patients' performance, even those with minimal cognitive deficits.
Cognitive rehabilitation based on the relative implicit memory spare to assist the ADL
Cognitive rehabilitation (CR) is composed of techniques and strategies that aim for minimizing deleterious effects originated by lesion or dysfunction of cognitive functions77. These functions are seen as a support for primary mental activities, e.g., memory, attention, thought, language, logic reasoning, etc. In this sense, CR strategies are used to compensate for the deficits caused in the ADL. In relation to AD, the CR focuses on minimizing the existing deficits due to the deterioration of memory systems. Particularly, it is seen that patients in mild to moderate stages of AD firstly show a degradation of the explicit memory, while implicit memory stays preserved for a longer time. In this way, recent studies have shown an increase in use of rehabilitation strategies which establish an association between explicit and implicit memory78-80. In this sense, Light and Singh78 compared young and elderly individuals in two different tasks. The first, an explicit task (aided recall from the first 3 letters of word, e.g., "ant...") and the second an implicit task (word completion from the radical formed by the first 3 letters of word, e.g., "ant..."). Although, the auxiliary elements were identical on the tasks, the procedures were different. The subjects received specific instructions about how they had been doing the tasks. The results demonstrated a better performance regarding the young subjects of 30% compared with elderly subjects during the aided recall and, a difference around 6% during the word completion task.
Such evidence focused on the possibility that therapeutic strategies are based on the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge as the disease progress. A possible therapeutic strategy is to link nonverbalized implicit knowledge to conscious effort when individuals are exposed to different tasks. Over the last years, two general intervention approaches and have been applied in AD patients with memory deficits. The first approach aims to work the residual skills of the compromised memory81. The other one intends to work the intact memory to compensate for the deficits of the compromised memory82.
The first approach intervenes through techniques like the Reality Orientation Therapy (ROT), which involves a continued and organized presentation of data. Its objective is creating environmental stimuli that have eased the temporal and spatial orientation of the patient83. Other technique employed in this same approach is the reminiscent therapy (RT). RT aims to stimulate the recall of mnemonic information through figures, pictures, musics, games and other stimuli related to patient youth84. A third technique supported itself on external aids as agendas, notes, alarm clocks, posters and signals aiming to compensate for the memory deficits that cannot be directly faced85.
In relation to second approach, supported by the compensation of the implicit memory, several techniques as errorless and sensorimotor learning are useful to learn and retain new information by mild AD patients86. As for the first technique, it was reported that errors made by patients during the learning process impair the retention of information87. This mechanism error-free based on a dynamic in favor of the elaboration of new implicit memory. According to Wilson88, individuals with amnesic deficits in the episodic memory, as AD patients, are not able to remember their own errors, thus they do not learn as subjects without memory deficits.
It is knowledge that through repetitive practice and mechanisms of implicit memory, AD patients can be trained for complex tasks89. However, it is due to the strict and specific features of implicit learning, that the patients cannot using their learning in flexibly way. In this sense, it suggested that will have been doing training based on specific knowledge with direct application in ADL90. All available studies reviewed on this matter18-21 show that AD patients learn best under constant practice conditions. According to Dick et al.18, humans use their episodic memory of the training trials to accurately perform a task while learning a skill. They suggest that because AD patients experience problems with episodic memory, constant practice is more effective because repeated running of the same motor program does not require an intact episodic memory.
In relation to sensorimotor learning technique, AD patients seem to benefit from multisensory situations where associations of verbal, visual and kinesthetic cues are enhanced and conditioned through particular circumstances85. Therefore, feedback is important for procedural learning in the AD patients. In this sense, there are two types of feedback, intrinsic where the sensory information is provided by motion and extrinsic where information comes from an external source like a verbal command. It is common knowledge that constant feedback enhances only motor performance, in contrast to the level of learning. The lack of feedback entails in the dependence on other cues, which provide more elaborate encoding. Numerous investigations related to motor-skill learning in AD patients employed visual feedback. Nevertheless, just the Maze tasks were performed according to blindfolded conditions. In these experiments, it was observed that the amount of learning in the AD patients was inferior compared with the controls48,49.
Other experiments (Rotor-Pursuit task) individualized the velocity of the target to equate initial performance. According to these experiments, it was verified that controls generally tracked at a faster rate than the AD patients54,55,63,64. Perhaps, AD patients can just perform the task at a slower rate due to their dependence on visual feedback more than controls. In this manner, only one Rotor-Pursuit experiment in explicit form examined the role of visual feedback on performance in AD patients. It was verified a decrease in performance when the visibility of the moving target was diminished during learning53. In opposition to the controls, the performance of patients did not pick up through trials in the restricted-vision condition. In the full-vision condition AD patients showed normal learning. It appears that constant visual feedback is important in learning and retention (procedural memory) for AD patients. However, it was not found any studies concerning the frequency of external feedback.
In a longitudinal study involving 3 probable AD patients, it was investigated the effects of a cognitive rehabilitation program. This program aims to work the residual explicit and implicit memories through ADL, compensation strategies and cognitive skills still preserved. Such findings indicate cognitive improvement, functional stabilization and reduced behavioral problems after the first year of the neuropsychological rehabilitation program. However, this improvement was not observed on second year, due to disease's progression77.
FINAL REMARKS
Concluding, our review yielded the following significant findings. In relation to first question, the results showed that preserved implicit memory irrespective of the task used, i.e., it not interference from explicit memory. AD patients are capable of learning without awareness simply by repeated exposure, although their performances will not reach normal levels. This is expressed in their prolonged performance when compared with unimpaired controls. Moreover, the extent of learning will differ depending on the task to be skilled. The reviewed studies showed that implicit learning, constant, or rather frequent and consistent practice is important for AD patients. This way of learning draws less on episodic memory and other cognitive functions compromised in AD patients. These results also suggest that practice under dual-task conditions should also be avoided. In addition, the amount of training needed by a patient will depend on the task being performed.
In relation to second question, the results showed that is possible through implicit memory compensatory strategies (i.e., ADL) to compensate for the explicit memory deficits. However, the effects of massed and distributed practice in this generally older patient group need to be addressed in future investigations. AD patients appear to remain dependent upon visual feedback throughout training and performance. The type and point in time when external feedback needs to be given and its effect on learning in AD also warrants attention in future research. In this sense, cognitive rehabilitation based on practice and feedback principles demonstrates reasonable results; with more highlight for second approach. Therefore, AD patients can use preserved implicit learning capacities in cognitive rehabilitation programs in order to compensate for explicit memory deficits.
Thus, the decline in explicit memory suggests that both systems are functionally independent and one can function normally even if the other is compromised. Patients with more impaired explicit memory system performed better on the implicit learning task. A possible speculation is that impairment in the explicit memory system results in better performance on the implicit memory task due to elimination of competition between the systems. We expect that when explicit memory system is not involved in competition with the implicit system, the final effect of learning is better, because all of the implicit memory capacity is engaged in learning and not in competition with the explicit system.
Received 21 August 2008, received in final form 27 November 2008. Accepted 21 February 2009.
Dr. Sergio Eduardo de Carvalho Machado - Rua Professor Sabóia Ribeiro 69/104 - 22430-130 Rio de Janeiro RJ - Brasil. E-mail: secm80@yahoo.com.br
References
- 1. Schacter DL, Slotnick SD. The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion. Neuron 2004; 44:149-160.
- 2. Fell J, Fernández G, Klaver P, et al. Rhinal-hippocampal coupling during declarative memory formation: dependence on item characteristics. Neurosci Lett 2006; 407:37-41.
- 3. Hirtz D, Thurman DJ, Gwinn-Hardy K, Mohamed M, Chaudhuri AR, Zalutsky R. How common are the "common" neurologic disorders? Neurology 2007; 68:326-337.
- 4. Klimkowicz-Mrowiec A, Slowik A, Krzywoszanski L, Herzog-Krzywoszanska R, Szczudlik A. Severity of explicit memory impairment due to Alzheimer's disease improves effectiveness of implicit learning. J Neurol 2008; 255:502-509.
- 5. Fleischman DA, Wilson RS, Gabrieli JD, Schneider JA, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Implicit memory and Alzheimer's disease neuropathology. Brain 2005; 128:2006-2015.
- 6. Braak H, Braak E. Evolution of the neuropathology of Alzheimer's disease. Acta Neurol Scand 1996; 165(Suppl):S3-S12.
- 7. Fox NC, Schott JM. Imaging cerebral atrophy: normal ageing to Alzheimer's disease. Lancet 2004; 363:392-394.
- 8. Herholz K. PET studies in dementia. Ann Nucl Med 2003; 17:79-89.
- 9. Pihlajamäki M, DePeau KM, Blacker D, Sperling RA. Impaired medial temporal repetition suppression is related to failure of parietal deactivation in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 16:283-292.
- 10. Petrella JR, Wang L, Krishnan S, et al. Cortical deactivation in mild cognitive impairment: high-field-strength functional MR imaging. Radiology 2007; 245:224-235.
- 11. Sperling R. Functional MRI studies of associative encoding in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1097:146-155.
- 12. Farrow TF, Thiyagesh SN, Wilkinson ID, Parks RW, Ingram L, Woodruff PW. Fronto-temporal-lobe atrophy in early-stage Alzheimer's disease identified using an improved detection methodology. Psychiatry Res 2007; 155:11-19.
- 13. Sperling R, Bates J, Chua E, et al. fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003; 74:44-50.
- 14. Poldrack RA, Clark J, Pare-Blagoev EJ, et al. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature 2001; 414:546-550.
- 15. Hotermans C, Peigneux P, Maertens de Noordhout A, Moonen G, Maquet P. Early boost and slow consolidation in motor skill learning. Learn Mem 2006; 13: 580-583.
- 16. Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR. Preserved motor learning in Alzheimer's disease: Implications for anatomy and behavior. J Neurosci 1986; 6:3006-3009.
- 17. Gabrieli JD, Corkin S, Mickel SF, Growdon JH. Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease and in global amnesia. Behav Neurosci 1993; 107:899-910.
- 18. Dick MB, Shankle RW, Beth RE, Dick-Muehlke C, Cotman CW, Kean ML. Acquisition and long-term retention of a gross motor skill in Alzheimer's disease patients under constant and varied practice conditions. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1996; 51:103-111.
- 19. Dick MB, Hsieh S, Dick-Muehlke C, Davis DS, Cotman CW. The variability of practice hypothesis in motor learning: does it apply to Alzheimer's disease? Brain Cogn 2000; 44:470-489.
- 20. Dick MB, Andel R, Hsieh S, Bricker J, Davis DS, Dick-Muehlke C. Contextual interference and motor skill learning in Alzheimer's disease. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2000; 7:273-287.
- 21. Dick MB, Hsieh S, Bricker J, Dick-Muelhlke C. Facilitating acquisition of a continuous motor task in healthy older adults and patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology 2003;17:202-212.
- 22. Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 1995;269:1880-1882.
- 23. Tin C, Poon CS. Internal models in sensorimotor integration: perspectives from adaptive control theory. J Neural Eng 2005; 2:S147-163.
- 24. Denève S, Duhamel JR, Pouget A. Optimal sensorimotor integration in recurrent cortical networks: a neural implementation of Kalman filters. J Neurosci 2007; 27:5744-5756.
- 25. Beauregard M, Benhamou J, Laurent C, Chertkow H. Word priming without awareness: a new approach to circumvent explicit memory contamination. Brain Cogn 1999; 39:149-169.
- 26. Poldrack RA, Packard MG. Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41:45-51.
- 27. Fleischman DA. Repetition priming in aging and Alzheimer's disease: an integrative review and future directions. Cortex 2007; 43:889-897.
- 28. Schacter DL, Wig GS, Stevens WD. Reductions in cortical activity during priming. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2007;17:171-176.
- 29. Mitchell DB, Bruss PJ. Age differences in implicit memory: conceptual, perceptual, or methodological? Psychol Aging 2003; 18:807-822.
- 30. Schott BH, Henson RN, Richardson-Klavehn A, et al. Redefining implicit and explicit memory: the functional neuroanatomy of priming, remembering, and control of retrieval. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2005; 102:1257-1262.
- 31. Gupta P, Cohen NJ. Theoretical and computational analysis of skill learning, repetition priming, and procedural memory. Psychol Rev 2002; 109:401-448.
- 32. Sui J, Han S. Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychol Sci 2007; 18:861-866.
- 33. Soldan A, Gazes E, Hilton JH, Stern Y. Aging does not affect brain patterns of repetition effects associated with perceptual priming of novel objects. J Cogn Neurosci 2008, in press.
- 34. Newell BR, Cavenett T, Andrews S. On the immunity of perceptual implicit memory to manipulations of attention. Mem Cogn 2008; 36:725-734.
- 35. Morgado I. The psychobiology of learning and memory: fundamentals and recent advances. Rev Neurol 2005; 40:289-297.
- 36. Hadj-Bouziane F, Meunier M, Boussaoud D. Conditional visuo-motor learning in primates: a key role for the basal ganglia. J Physiol (Paris) 2003; 97:567-579.
- 37. Guimarães HC, Levy R, Teixeira AL, Beato RG, Caramelli P. Neurobiology of apathy in Alzheimer's disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2008; 66:436-443.
- 38. Camus JF, Wenisch NE, Blanchard MF, Bakchine S. Implicit memory for words presented in short texts is preserved in Alzheimer's disease? Psychol Med 2003; 33:169-174.
- 39. Golby A, Silverberg G, Race E, et al. Memory encoding in Alzheimer's disease: an fMRI study of explicit and implicit memory. Brain 2005; 128:773-787.
- 40. Eldridge LL, Masterman D, Knowlton BJ. Intact implicit habit learning in Alzheimer's disease. Behav Neurosci 2002;116:722-726.
- 41. Harrison BE, Son GR, Kim J, Whall AL. Preserved implicit memory in dementia: a potential model for care. Am J Alzheimer's Dis Other Demen 2007; 22:286-293.
- 42. Martins CA, Lloyd-Jones TJ. Preserved conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 2006;42:995-1004.
- 43. Cuetos F, Gonzalez-Nosti M, Martínez C. The picturenaming task in the analysis of cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer's disease. Aphasiology 2005; 19:545-557.
- 44. Ramponi C, Richardson-Klavehn A, Gardiner JM. Component processes of conceptual priming and associative cued recall: the roles of preexisting representation and depth of processing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2007;33:843-862.
- 45. Voss JL, Reber PJ, Mesulam MM, Parrish TB, Paller KA. Familiarity and conceptual priming engage distinct cortical networks. Cereb Cortex 2008; 18:1712-1719.
- 46. Karlsson T, Borjesson A, Adolfsson R, Nilsson LG. Successive memory test performance and priming in Alzheimer's disease: evidence from the word-fragment completion task. Cortex 2002;38:341-355.
- 47. Rouleau I, Salmon DP, Vrbancic M. Learning, retention and generalization of a mirror tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002; 24:239-250.
- 48. Kuzis G, Sabe L, Tiberti C, Merello M, Leiguarda G, Starkstein SE. Explicit and implicit learning in patients with Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease with dementia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1999; 12:265-269.
- 49. Sabe L, Jason L, Juejati M, Leigarda R, Starkstein SE. Dissociations between declarative and procedural learning in dementia and depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995;17:841-848.
- 50. Starkstein SE, Sabe L., Cuerva AG, Kuzis G, Leigarda R. Anosognosia and procedural learning in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1997; 10:96-101.
- 51. Taylor R. Spiral maze performance in dementia. Percept Mot Skills 1998;87:328-330.
- 52. Beatty WW, Scott JG, Wilson DA, Prince RJ, Williamson DJ. Memory deficits in a demented patient with probable corticobasal degeneration. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1995; 8: 132-137.
- 53. Dick MB, Andel R, Bricker J, Gorospe JB, Hsieh S, Dick-Muehlke C. Dependence on visual feedback during motor skill learning in Alzheimer's disease. Aging, Neuropsychol Cogn 2001;8:120-136.
- 54. Dick MB, Nielson KA, Beth RB, Shankle WR, Cotman CW. Acquisition and long-term retention of a fine motor skill an Alzheimer's disease. Brain Cogn 1995; 29:294-306.
- 55. Jacobs DH, Adair JC, Williamson DJG, et al. Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimer's disease are dissociable. Neuropsychologia 1999; 37:875-880.
- 56. Willingham BB, Peterson EW, Manning C, Brashear HR. Patients with Alzheimer's disease who cannot perform some motor skills show normal learning of other motor skills. Neuropsychology 1997; 11:261-271.
- 57. Poe MK, Seifert LS. Implicit and explicit tests: evidence for dissociable motor skills in probable Alzheimer's dementia. Percept Mot Skills 1997; 85:631-634.
- 58. Knopman DS, Nissen MJ. Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1987; 37:784-788.
- 59. Grafman J, Weingartner H, Newhouse PA, et al. Implicit learning in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Pharmacopsychiatry 1990; 23:94-101.
- 60. Knopman, D. Long-term retention of implicitly acquired learning in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1991; 13:880-894.
- 61. Ferraro FR, Balota DA, Connor LT. Implicit memory and the formation of new associations in non-demented Parkinson's disease individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: a serial reaction time (SRT) investigation. Brain Cogn 1993; 21:163-180.
- 62. Hirono N, Mori E, Ikejiri Y, et al. Procedural memory in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1997; 8:210-216.
- 63. Libon DJ, Bogdanoff B, Cloud BS, et al. Declarative and procedural learning, quantitative measures of the hippocampus, and subcortical white alterations in Alzheimer's disease and Ischaemic Vascular dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1998; 20:30-41.
- 64. Deweer B, Ergis AM, Fosatti P, et al. Explicit memory, procedural learning and lexical priming in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 1994; 30:113-126.
- 65. Heindel WC, Butters N, Salmon DP. Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with Huntington's disease. Behav Neurosci 1988; 102:141-147.
- 66. Heindel WC, Salmon DP, Shults W, Walicke PA, Butters N. Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients. J Neurosci 1989; 9:582-587.
- 67. Mayr U. Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem Cogn 1996;22:350-364.
- 68. Petersen SE, Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL. PET study of parietal involvement in spatial attention: comparison of different task types. Can J Exp Psychol 1994; 48:319-338.
- 69. Deiber MP, Wise SP, Honda M, Catalan MJ, Grafman J, Hallett M. Frontal and parietal networks for conditional motor learning: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurophysiol 1997; 78:977-991.
- 70. Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. Adaptive representations of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci 1994; 4: 3208-3225.
- 71. Tse DW, Spaulding SJ. Review of motor control and motor learning: implications for occupational therapy with individuals with Parkinson's disease. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr 1998; 15:19-38.
- 72. Willingham DB, Goedert-Eschmann K. The relation between implicit and explicit learning: evidence for parallel development. Psychol Sci 1999; 10:531-534.
- 73. Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Motor learning and performance: a situation-based learning approach. 4 edition. USA: Human Kinetics, 2007:282-318.
- 74. Schmidt RA. A schema theory of discrete motor skills. Psychol Rev 1975; 82:225-260.
- 75. Schmidt RA. Motor schema theory after 27 years: reflections and implications for a new theory. Res Q Exerc Sport 2003; 74:366-375.
- 76. Tippett WJ, Sergio LE. Visuomotor integration is impaired in early stage Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res 2006;1102:92-102.
- 77. Abrisqueta-Gomez J, Canali F, Vieira V, et al. A longitudinal study of a neuropsychological rehabilitation program in Alzheimer's disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2004; 62:778-783.
- 78. Light LL, Singh A. Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1987; 13: 531-541.
- 79. Park DC, Shaw RJ. Effect of environmental support on implicit and explicit memory in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging 1992; 7:632-642.
- 80. Davis HP, Bernstein PA. Age-related changes in explicit and implicit memory. In: Squire LR, Butters N (Eds.). Neuropsychology of memory. 2Ş edition. New York: The Guilford Press, 1992:249-261.
- 81. Wilson BA. Reabilitação das deficiências cognitivas. In: Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Mansur LL (Eds.). Neuropsicologia das bases anatômicas à reabilitação. São Paulo: Clínica Neurológica HCFMUSP, 1996:314-343.
- 82. Bayles K, Kim E. Improving the functioning of individuals with Alzheimer's disease: emergence of behavioral interventions. J Commun Disord 2003; 36:327-343.
- 83. Onder G, Zanetti O, Giacobini E, et al. Reality orientation therapy combined with cholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer's disease: randomised controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 187: 450-455.
- 84. Okumura Y, Tanimukai S, Asada T. Effects of short-term reminiscence therapy on elderly with dementia: a comparison with everyday conversation approaches. Psychogeriatrics 2008;8:124-133.
- 85. Ávila R, Miotto E. Reabilitação neuropsicológica de déficits de memória em pacientes com demência de Alzheimer. Rev Psiq Clín 2002; 29:190-196.
- 86. Clare L, Jones RS. Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impairment: a critical review. Neuropsychol Rev 2008;18:1-23.
- 87. Brooks BM, Rose FD. The use of virtual reality in memory rehabilitation: current findings and future directions. NeuroRehabilitation. 2003; 18:147-157.
- 88. Wilson BA, Baddeley AD, Evans JJ, Shiel A. Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people. Neuropsycholl Rehabil 1994; 4:307-326.
- 89. Cavaco S, Anderson SW, Allen JS, Castro-Caldas A, Damasio H. The scope of preserved procedural memory in amnesia. Brain 2004; 127:1853-1867.
- 90. Bottino CM, Carvalho IA, Alvarez AM, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in Alzheimer's disease patients: multidisciplinary team report. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2002; 60:70-79.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
05 June 2009 -
Date of issue
June 2009
History
-
Accepted
21 Feb 2009 -
Received
27 Nov 2008