Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Fiddler crabs from highly disturbed beaches are more sensitive to human presence

Abstract

The presence of humans frequently modifies the behavior of animals, particularly their foraging patterns, compromising energetic demands. The fiddler crab Leptuca leptodactyla inhabits mangroves with high degrees of anthropogenic influence. Thus, we tested if populations living in highly anthropized mangroves respond differently from those living in more protected areas. We predict that individuals from touristy areas will be more tolerant to humans and will resume their activities sooner after disturbance. To do so, we conducted an experiment that consisted in the approach of an observer to the burrows, recording the response of individuals to the stimuli. The experiment took place in July 2022, in Ubatuba, São Paulo, Brazil. We analysed the duration and latency of various behaviors of a total of 80 adult males from two populations (high and low anthropogenic influence). Contrary to our predictions, individuals from the anthropized population were less tolerant, spending more time inside their burrows and taking longer to resume their activities. Therefore, fiddler crabs were not habituated to human presence. These results help us understand the learning process in invertebrates and their ability to select stimuli, contributing to understanding the impacts of human-wildlife interactions.

Key words
Anthropogenic disturbances; behavior; latency; time budget; Leptuca

INTRODUCTION

Wildlife responses to anthropogenic disturbances are complex and influenced by several factors (Bejder et al. 2009BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, FINN H & ALLEN S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177-185.). The exponential development of cities and urbanization have resulted in an increasing alteration of natural environments (Hamer & McDonnell 2009HAMER AJ & MCDONNELL MJ. 2009. The response of herpetofauna to urbanization: Inferring patterns of persistence from wildlife databases. Austral Ecol 35: 568-580.), increasing the frequency of interactions and conflicts between human populations and wildlife (Abrahams et al. 2018ABRAHAMS MI, PERES CA & COSTA HCM. 2018. Manioc losses by terrestrial vertebrates in western Brazilian Amazonia. J Wildl Manage 82: 734-746., Walton et al. 2022WALTON BJ, FINDLAY LJ & HILL RA. 2022. Camera traps and guard observations as an alternative to researcher observation for studying anthropogenic foraging. Ecol Evol 12: e8808.). With the loss of natural resources and habitat and high degrees of anthropogenic disturbances, the most sensitive species tend to disappear, while less sensitive species, depending on the level of degradation/environmental alteration, manage to resist these changes and coexist with humans (McKinney 2006MCKINNEY ML. 2006. Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homogenization. Biol Conserv 127: 247-260.). This flexibility can be observed at the behavioral level, where individuals that can adjust their behavior are more likely to tolerate human presence (Sih et al. 2011SIH A, FERRARI MCO & HARRIS DJ. 2011. Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evol Appl 4: 367-387., Lowry et al. 2013LOWRY H, LILL A & WONG BBM. 2013. Behavioural responses of wildlife to urban environments. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 88: 537-549., Sol et al. 2013SOL D, LAPIEDRA O & GONZÁLEZ-LAGOS C. 2013. Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Anim Behav 85: 1101-1112.).

On one side, animals can present the so-called habituation, a gradual reduction in responsiveness to recurrent events (Rankin et al. 2009RANKIN ET AL. 2009. Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92: 135-138.). As the direct opposite of habituation, there is a phenomenon called sensitization (Peeke 1984PEEKE H. 1984. Habituation, Sensitization, and Behavior, Elsevier, 486 p.), defined as the intensification of the response to stimuli. Both phenomena are related to the stimuli and how it perceived by the animal in question, low intensity and high volumes of stimuli tend to habituate, while high intensity and low volumes tend to sensitize (Blumstein 2016BLUMSTEIN DT. 2016. Habituation and sensitization: new thoughts about old ideas. Anim Behav 120: 255-262.). These processes are relevant to be addressed given that some animals might habituate to the human presence or sensitize to it (Blumstein 2016BLUMSTEIN DT. 2016. Habituation and sensitization: new thoughts about old ideas. Anim Behav 120: 255-262.). For instance, some species do not perceive humans as predators, becoming habituated to them (Walker 1972WALKER I. 1972. Habituation to disturbance in the fiddler crab (Uca annulipes) in its natural environment. Anim Behav 20: 139-146., MacFarlane & King 2002MACFARLANE GR & KING SA. 2002. Observer presence influences behaviour of the semaphore crab, Heloecious cordiformis. Anim Behav 63: 1191-1194., Knight 2009KNIGHT J. 2009. Making Wildlife Viewable: Habituation and Attraction. Soc Anim 17: 167-184.). Such capacity demonstrates that animals can filter non-threatening stimuli from those necessary to survival, avoiding unnecessary energy expenditure (Geist 2011GEIST V. 2011. advances in understanding and management application. Hum-wildl interact 5: 9-12., Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388., Raderschall et al. 2011RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216.). Filtering certain stimuli allows for increased foraging time and decreased periods of alertness; thus, being an advantage to the individual (Hemmi 2005bHEMMI JM. 2005b. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 2. The visual cues. Anim Behav 69: 615-625.).

Fiddler crabs comprise several genera of crabs of the Ocypodidae family that have an extensive distribution around the world, being present on all continents except Antarctica (Crane 1975CRANE J. 1975. Fiddler crabs of the world. Ocypodidae: Genus Uca, New Jersey: Princepton University Press, p. 304-308., Rosenberg 2019ROSENBERG MS. 2019. A fresh look at the biodiversity lexicon for fiddler crabs (Decapoda: Brachyura: Ocypodidae). Part 1: Taxonomy. J Crustacean Biol: 729-738.). Due to their wide distribution (Rosenberg 2019ROSENBERG MS. 2019. A fresh look at the biodiversity lexicon for fiddler crabs (Decapoda: Brachyura: Ocypodidae). Part 1: Taxonomy. J Crustacean Biol: 729-738.), fiddler crabs frequently interact or reside on beaches with human presence, thus enabling studies on the effects of human presence on essential behaviors, such as feeding and fleeing (Zeil & Hemmi 2006ZEIL J & HEMMI JM. 2006. The visual ecology of fiddler crabs. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 192: 1-25.). Studies with species worldwide have demonstrated that fiddler crabs can filter various stimuli and exhibit habituation to humans and cars (Walker 1972WALKER I. 1972. Habituation to disturbance in the fiddler crab (Uca annulipes) in its natural environment. Anim Behav 20: 139-146., Tomsic et al. 1993TOMSIC D, MASSONI V & MALDONADO H. 1993. Habituation to a danger stimulus in two semiterrestrial crabs: ontogenic, ecological and opioid modulation correlates. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 173., Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388., Raderschall et al. 2011RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216., DiNuzzo et al. 2020DINUZZO ER, ANDERSON L III, WALKER A, SASSO HK, CHRISTENSEN B & GRIFFEN BD. 2020. Human influences on male waving behavior in the fiddler crab Leptuca pugilator. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol 53: 43-57.). However, some studies found that habituation occurred only in certain contexts (Tomsic et al. 1998TOMSIC D, PEDREIRA ME, ROMANO A, HERMITTE G & MALDONADO H. 1998. Context-us association as a determinant of long-term habituation in the crabChasmagnathus. Anim Learn Behav 26: 196-209., Park & Kim 2021PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178.).

The target species is Leptuca leptodactyla, a species commonly found along the Brazilian coast (Thurman et al. 2013THURMAN CL, FARIA SC & MCNAMARA JC. 2013. The distribution of fiddler crabs (Uca) along the coast of Brazil: implications for biogeography of the western Atlantic Ocean. Mar Biodivers Rec 6: e1.), usually in high densities and in areas with low vegetation cover (Checon & Costa 2017CHECON HH & COSTA TM. 2017. Fiddler crab (Crustacea: Ocypodidae) distribution and the relationship between habitat occupancy and mouth appendages. Mar Biol Res 13: 618-629.). This species occupies mangroves, one of the most productive and threatened ecosystems, suffering acutely with the expansion of human settlements (Angelini et al. 2018ANGELINI C, VAN MONTFRANS SG, HENSEL MJS, HE Q & SILLIMAN BR. 2018. The importance of an underestimated grazer under climate change: how crab density, consumer competition, and physical stress affect salt marsh resilience. Oecologia 187: 205-217., Leonardi et al. 2018LEONARDI N, CARNACINA I, DONATELLI C, GANJU NK, PLATER AJ, SCHUERCH M & TEMMERMAN S. 2018. Dynamic interactions between coastal storms and salt marshes: A review. Geomorphology (Amst) 301: 92-107.), with a deforestation rate of 1 to 2% a year (Alongi 2015ALONGI DM. 2015. The impact of climate change on mangrove forests. Curr Clim Change Rep 1: 30-39.), emphasizing the necessity of understanding the impacts of humans on the local fauna. The present study aimed to test the hypothesis that the behavior of fiddler crabs, L. leptodactyla, will be modified by human presence. In the area with higher anthropogenic influence, we expect fewer individuals to respond to an observer’s approach and, when they respond, they will take longer to respond, will spend less time in their burrows and take less time to resume foraging. Our study adds to the understanding of learning in invertebrates and their selection of stimuli, as well as contributes to the understanding of the influence of human populations on wildlife.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Location of study and characterization of the beaches

The study was conducted in Ubatuba, São Paulo, Brazil, in the mangrove of Praia Dura/Rio Escuro (23°29’3” S, 45°09’5” W) (Fig. 1). We sampled two populations living in two different beaches inside this mangrove separated by approximately 160m. Populations were chosen based on observable differences in anthropogenic influence, considering access and logistical limitations. In the beach with the highest frequency of tourists, there is a bridge 75 m afar acting as a barrier for tourist passage. We used this construction to delimit one of the populations and employed the same distance to the other.

Figure 1
Image obtained by Google Maps showing the two beaches where the study was conducted. High anthropogenic influence (HAI), Low anthropogenic influence (LAI). Source: Google Maps.

To assess the degree of anthropogenic influence, we followed the criteria of González et al. (2014)GONZÁLEZ SA, YÁÑEZ-NAVEA K & MUÑOZ M. 2014. Effect of coastal urbanization on sandy beach coleoptera Phaleria maculata (Kulzer, 1959) in northern Chile. Mar Pollut Bull 83: 265-274., with adaptations, employing six out of seven urbanization variables proposed, namely 1) number of tourists; 2) proximity to urban centers; 3) buildings on the sand; 4) beach cleaning from vehicles; 5) solid garbage in the sand; 6) vehicle traffic on the sand. Each of the above cited variables was assigned a value of zero (low) to five (high). For the number of tourists, since we did not have access to the data from local authorities, we used field observation only, considered an adequate method for counting (Morgan 2018MORGAN D. 2018. Counting Beach Visitors: Tools, Methods and Management Applications. In: BOTERO CM et al. (Eds), Beach Management Tools - Concepts, Methodologies and Case Studies, Cham: Springer International Publishing, p. 561-577.). Field observation consisted of the manual counting of tourist numbers during the peak hours of recreational activities, considering everyone found within the 75 m radius of each fiddler crab population, following a similar rationale for the nearby buildings (see below). Proximity from urban centers was calculated with Google Maps based on the nearest residential neighbourhood. Buildings on the sand were considered those near the populations, and the bridge was the only building near these populations. Mechanical cleaning did not happen for any of the beaches. The solid garbage in the sand was quantified by two parallel transects of 30 m (4 m apart). Garbage counting happened on Monday after the weekend when the experiment was conducted. The vehicle traffic on the sand is forbidden in the area (Table I) of the values of these six variables were included in the urbanization index calculation which gives a result ranging from 0 to 1, where zero indicates beaches with lower urbanization index and 1 indicates higher urbanization index. The mangrove beach of Praia Dura/Rio Escuro presented an urbanization index of 0.3 while the other presented a value of 0.066. Based on these values, the beaches were separated into low anthropogenic influence (LAI) and high anthropogenic influence (HAI) following their urbanization index (low or high). Therefore, animals living on the beach with a higher level of anthropogenic influence would be more exposed to human presence than animals living on beaches with a lower level of influence.

Table I
Ethogram made for adult male L. leptodactyla living in Ubatuba, São Paulo based on van Himbeeck et al. (2019)VAN HIMBEECK RAF, HUIZINGA W, ROESSINK I & PEETERS ETHM. 2019. Behavioral patterns of two fiddler crab species Uca rapax and Uca tangeri in a seminatural mangrove system. Zoo Biol 38: 343-354..

Study animals

Species identification followed the parameters of Crane (1975)CRANE J. 1975. Fiddler crabs of the world. Ocypodidae: Genus Uca, New Jersey: Princepton University Press, p. 304-308., de Melo (1996)DE MELO GAS. 1996. Manual de identificação dos brachyura (caranguejos e siris) do litoral brasileiro, Editora Plêiade, 603 p., and Rosenberg (2019)ROSENBERG MS. 2019. A fresh look at the biodiversity lexicon for fiddler crabs (Decapoda: Brachyura: Ocypodidae). Part 1: Taxonomy. J Crustacean Biol: 729-738., so that the study species was identified as L. leptodactyla. The individuals could easily be separated according to sex due to the developed heterochelia in males (Crane 1975CRANE J. 1975. Fiddler crabs of the world. Ocypodidae: Genus Uca, New Jersey: Princepton University Press, p. 304-308.). Adult males also have an average size of 5 mm in length, although some individuals can reach 6.5mm (Crane 1975CRANE J. 1975. Fiddler crabs of the world. Ocypodidae: Genus Uca, New Jersey: Princepton University Press, p. 304-308.). Crabs could also be identified individually, as each burrow was used by a single individual (Hemmi 2005aHEMMI JM. 2005a. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 1. Escape decisions in relation to the risk of predation. Anim Behav 69: 603-614.). Additionally, since crab behavior can vary according to sex (Chumsri et al. 2023CHUMSRI A, TINA FW, VONGKOK A, CHINNANON M, CHUAYTANEE N, ANGAJCHARIYA A & SUJARIT C. 2023. Human disturbance affects behavioural time allocation in a fiddler crab (Austruca annulipes) in Southern Thailand. J Anim Behav Biometeorol 11(3): 2023025.), we restricted our study to adult males.

Each population (high and low anthropogenic influence) was sampled twice (two subgroups of different individuals). The two subgroups distanced themselves by about 25m. In each of these subgroups, 20 randomly selected adult males were analysed, totalling 80 animals for the study (40 per population) (Fig. 2).

Figure 2
Image captured during the experiment, showing the counting area (mark on the sand) and adult males of Leptuca leptodactyla (white arrows).

Experiment

The experiment was conducted in July 2022, the winter break for the region, a month marked by an increase in the number of visitors by up to 500% (Poletto & Batista 2008). Such an increase in visitors can likely change the urbanization index and emphasize the differences between the two beaches, as seen in other sites (Asensio-Montesinos et al. 2019ASENSIO-MONTESINOS F, ANFUSO G, RANDERSON P & WILLIAMS AT. 2019. Seasonal comparison of beach litter on Mediterranean coastal sites (Alicante, SE Spain). Ocean Coast Manag 181: 104914., Vincent & Hoellein 2017VINCENT AES & HOELLEIN TJ. 2017. Anthropogenic litter abundance and accumulation rates point to seasonal litter sources on a Great Lakes Beach. Journal of Contemporary Water 160: 72-84.). Data collection involving the animals happened in a single day. We followed De Grande et al. (2018)DE GRANDE FR, GRANADO P, SANCHES FHC & COSTA TM. 2018. Organic matter affects fiddler crab distribution? Results from field and laboratorial trials. Estuar Coast Shelf Sci 212: 138-145. protocol where the date chosen for data collection had to be sunny and not preceded by storms. Moreover, data collection should not exceed 30 minutes for each population to avoid significant environmental changes.

We ensured that there was no interference from tourists during the experiment. The experiment consisted of a single approach by an observer with 1.95m in height. A camera (iPhone 8), fixed on a tripod with a total height of 80 cm and an angle of 45° towards the ground, recorded an area of 1.2 m². A period of 10 minutes of acclimatization was given to the crabs to avoid possible biased responses to the camera’s instalment (Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388.).

After the recording started, the observer waited for 10 s to start the approach. The approach lengthened 10 m from the recorded areas, ending at the limits of the camera’s field of view, taking 10 s to reach the end. After reaching the finishing point, the observer returned to the starting point using the same route. The observer walked at the same speed and wore the same clothing in all approaches, ensuring standardization in all experiments (Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388.). The same protocol was used for all subgroups. The recording lasted 190 seconds, like the procedure of Hemmi & Merkle (2009)HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388..

Ethogram

Our ethogram consisted of eight categories (Table I), with six behaviors directly related to burrows, such as fleeing (to burrow), entering the burrow, inside the burrow, emerging, unanchoring, and constructing (or burrow maintenance). Additionally, feeding behavior was split between pre-stimulus and post-stimulus. This conceptual division of the same behavior enabled a more refined understanding of foraging, an activity that represents a state of normality for populations when not stimulated, and the primary behavior represented in daily activity budgets (Weis & Weis 2004WEIS JS & WEIS P. 2004. Behavior of Four Species of Fiddler Crabs, Genus Uca, in Southeast Sulawesi, Indonesia. Hydrobiologia 523: 47-58.).

Feeding is the main activity performed by fiddler crabs because even during different activities, they feed simultaneously (Weis & Weis 2004WEIS JS & WEIS P. 2004. Behavior of Four Species of Fiddler Crabs, Genus Uca, in Southeast Sulawesi, Indonesia. Hydrobiologia 523: 47-58.). Therefore, feeding behavior is crucial to understand differences between populations. Pre-stimulus foraging duration represents the time elapsed until a response is noticed to the observer’s approach. Considering that all individuals were exposed to the same experiment (see below), a crab with a longer duration of pre-stimulus feeding means it tolerated the observer’s approach for longer (more tolerant individuals). As for post-stimulus feeding, the latency to initiate feeding indicates when the animal resumed its normal activities. Therefore, a more intense response to the observer’s approach translated into longer latency of the post-stimulus feeding behavior.

Video analysis

First, one of us (GBR) built an ethogram based on previously described behaviors (van Himbeeck et al. 2019VAN HIMBEECK RAF, HUIZINGA W, ROESSINK I & PEETERS ETHM. 2019. Behavioral patterns of two fiddler crab species Uca rapax and Uca tangeri in a seminatural mangrove system. Zoo Biol 38: 343-354.) with adaptations (Table I). Recordings were analyzed with BORIS software (Friard & Gamba 2016FRIARD O & GAMBA M. 2016. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol Evol 7: 1325-1330.). We listed the number of individuals that responded to the observer’s approach (flee in the direction of their burrow), the onset of each behavior, and its duration. In addition, we also measured the distance between the fiddler crabs and their respective burrows at the moment of the approach using ImageJ software (Schneider et al. 2012SCHNEIDER CA, RASBAND WS & ELICEIRI KW. 2012. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods 9: 671-675.). It is important to address that the camera was fixed at 45 degrees angle instead of 90 degrees as described by Hemmi (2005a, b), possibly causing some distortion in distances. Since some individuals were on the far end of the recorded frame and thus further away from the stimulus in question (approach of the observer), we separated the individuals into front and back blocks to assess whether there would be a difference in response between the two. No ethics approval was required to undertake this study.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using R software version 4.0.3 (R Development Core Team 2020). First, variables were tested for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Since all variables were non-normally distributed, we tested the difference in means with the Mann-Whitney U test and Dunn a posteriori test (FSA package). We also did a Pearson’s correlation between the position of the fiddler crab and the duration of it flee movement to assess if there was a direct relationship between the distance from the burrow and the time the crab took to reach it. The mean distance of crabs from their burrows was also tested for each beach (high and low anthropogenic influence). In addition, the significance of the differences in latency means between treatments was calculated for each of the behaviors using the Mann-Whitney U test and Dunn a posteriori test (FSA package) with latency values rounded to 10-second blocks, and the mean latency between populations treatments was calculated to elucidate the differences between treatments better.

RESULTS

Ethogram and populations differences

All individuals responded to the stimulus (the approach of the observer) by fleeing and entering their burrows. We found differences in behavior durations between populations for six of the eight categories, except for entering burrow and construction (Dunn’s Test p<0.001) (Table II), mostly because both behaviors were fast or rare (n=6 showed constructing behavior). The population with high anthropogenic influence spent more time inside their burrows, emerging and unanchoring, while the low anthropogenic influence population spent more time feeding (pre and post-stimulus) and fleeing.

Table II
Means (and standard deviations) in seconds for each behavior and distance, given in centimeters, for each population. HAI - High anthropogenic influence. LAI - Low anthropogenic influence. The behaviors marked with a * showed a significant difference between the two populations (p < 0.05).

Pearson’s correlation detected a strong and positive correlation between the flee duration and the individuals’ initial distance from their burrows (corr = 0.67; p<0.001). Therefore, animals that fled for longer were further from their burrows. As for latency, we did not detect differences between populations for entering the burrow (p=0.85), while all other behaviors showed similar latencies (p<0.05). Latencies of fleeing and entering the burrow were smaller in the high anthropogenic influence population, meaning that individuals from this population reacted sooner to the approach of the observer. Conversely, the low anthropogenic influence population had higher values of latency for emerging, unanchoring, and post-stimulus feeding, meaning that these individuals resumed their foraging activities sooner than the other population (Fig. 3).

Figure 3
Comparison of mean latencies, in seconds, for the main behaviors (flee, enter burrow, emerge from burrow and forage) analysed in adult males of Leptuca leptodactyla living on two beaches with different degrees of anthropization in the Praia Dura mangrove in Ubatuba, Brazil.

DISCUSSION

This work investigated whether the behavior of fiddler crabs, L. leptodactyla, was related to distinct urbanization index. Contrary to what we expected, all individuals responded to the observer approach. Moreover, the hypothesis of the present work was not supported since the crabs from the high anthropogenic influence beach presented intense escape responses to the observer’s approach and took longer to resume their pre-stimulus activities, such as foraging.

Feeding

All analyzed individuals were feeding before the stimulus, even those near the entrance of their burrows. The reaction to the approach of the observer caused the interruption of this behavior, a reaction like other fiddler crab species (Hemmi 2005aHEMMI JM. 2005a. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 1. Escape decisions in relation to the risk of predation. Anim Behav 69: 603-614.). A longer duration of pre-stimulus feeding was predicted in the HAI population, i.e., more tolerant individuals (Raderschall et al. 2011RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216.). Since the HAI population had constant interaction with humans, we expected that the approach of another human would not trigger such an intense response. Surprisingly, it was the beach with a lower urbanization index that showed higher averages of duration in this behavior (more tolerant), contesting the hypothesis of this work and the literature (Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388., Raderschall et al. 2011RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216.). In line with the results observed for pre-stimulus feeding, individuals from the LAI population also showed a longer duration of post-stimulus foraging behavior.

Scape behavior (flee)

Scape behavior was the only response observed in our population. We did not call this behavior a “home run” (Hemmi 2005aHEMMI JM. 2005a. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 1. Escape decisions in relation to the risk of predation. Anim Behav 69: 603-614., Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388.) because the definition of this behavior required a distance of at least 3 cm travelled towards the burrow. In addition, a strong correlation was detected between the distance from the burrow and the duration of the fleeing behavior, indicating that animals farther from their burrows would run for longer. Individuals from the LAI beach showed a greater distance from their burrows and longer fleeing behavior. These results contradict the literature, as individuals farther from their burrows are expected to react more quickly to stimuli (Hemmi & Zeil 2003HEMMI JM & ZEIL J. 2003. Burrow surveillance in fiddler crabs. II. The sensory cues. J Exp Biol 206: 3951-3961.).

Burrow-related behaviors

There were no differences between the mean durations of the two populations during the burrow entering. Since entering the burrow is a transitional behavior between being inside and outside and should be considered an event, not a state. Similarly, emerging was also a behavior that did not show significant differences between populations and should be treated as an event.

Staying inside the burrow is closely related to the post-stimulus feeding behavior, as all animals resumed foraging after leaving their burrows. Individuals from the HAI population spent more time inside their burrows (i.e., a more intense response) in comparison with the LAI (i.e., a less intense response). These results again contradict the literature, as a more intense response to the presence of humans was expected in HAI populations (Hemmi & Merkle 2009HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388., Raderschall et al. 2011RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216.). Since staying inside the burrow is associated with predator avoidance (Bellwood 2002BELLWOOD O. 2002. The occurrence, mechanics and significance of burying behaviour in crabs (Crustacea: Brachyura). J Nat Hist 36: 1223-1238., Jennions et al. 2003JENNIONS MD, BACKWELL PRY, MURAI M & CHRISTY JH. 2003. Hiding behaviour in fiddler crabs: how long should prey hide in response to a potential predator? Anim Behav 66: 251-257.) then perhaps the HAI population is perceiving humans as predators.

The unanchoring behavior is not considered in other studies on fiddler crabs (e.g., Hemmi 2005aHEMMI JM. 2005a. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 1. Escape decisions in relation to the risk of predation. Anim Behav 69: 603-614., b, Hemmi & Merkel 2009, Raderschall et al. 2011RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216.), although it showed great interpopulation variation. This behavior relates to the animal releasing its pereopods from any instance of its burrow. Hence, if animals are presenting shorter durations of this behavior, then they are distancing from their burrows more quickly. Unanchoring followed the same trend as other behaviors, where the HAI population presented more intense responses concerning the observer’s approach, with a higher mean duration in this population.

Possible explanations for non-habituation

There are several possible explanations for the phenomenon reported here (Bejder et al. 2009BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, FINN H & ALLEN S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177-185.); the effect observed here cannot be classified as habituation (Blumstein 2016BLUMSTEIN DT. 2016. Habituation and sensitization: new thoughts about old ideas. Anim Behav 120: 255-262.); dishabituation (Leclerc et al. 2017LECLERC M, ZEDROSSER A & PELLETIER F. 2017. Harvesting as a potential selective pressure on behavioural traits. J Appl Ecol 54: 1941-1945.); Harvesting as a potential selective pressure (Sadoul et al. 2021SADOUL B, BLUMSTEIN DT, ALFONSO S & GEFFROY B. 2021. Human protection drives the emergence of a new coping style in animals. PLoS Biol 19: e3001186.); Human presence altering selective forces), but we will discuss three of these possible explanations elucidated in the literature. The first explanation refers to the opposite effect of habituation, which is sensitization, defined as the increased response resulting from continuous exposure to repetitive stimuli (Peeke 1984PEEKE H. 1984. Habituation, Sensitization, and Behavior, Elsevier, 486 p.). Since the time of year in which we recorded the animals may have exacerbated the number of tourists, this scenario may have contributed to the fiddler crabs exhibiting sensitization instead of habituation, thus increasing their responsiveness to anthropogenic stimuli (Bejder et al. 2009BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, FINN H & ALLEN S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177-185.).

As another explanation is that crabs became sensitive in habitats with higher human pressure, which can be interpreted as high predation pressure. Kim et al. (2018)KIM TW, KIM S & LEE JA. 2018. Effects of mudflat trampling on activity of intertidal crabs. Ocean Science Journal 53: 101-106. and Park & Kim (2021)PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178. did a study with two crab species, conducting experiments similar to ours. They assessed crab behavior under different levels of human presence. Crabs living in areas with high trampling took more time to return to surface (stayed longer in their burrows) than those living in the area where visiting was prohibited. It is important to note that surface activities include significant behaviors, such as courtship (Park & Kim 2021PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178.) and feeding (Kim & Choe 2003KIM TW & CHOE JC. 2003. The effect of food availability on the semilunar courtship rhythm in the fiddler crab Uca lactea (de Haan) (Brachyura: Ocypodidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 54: 210-217., Kim et al. 2008KIM TW, SAKAMOTO K, HENMI Y & CHOE JC. 2008. To court or not to court: reproductive decisions by male fiddler crabs in response to fluctuating food availability. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62: 1139-1147., Park & Kim 2021PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178.). Another recent study from Thailand (Chumsri et al. 2023CHUMSRI A, TINA FW, VONGKOK A, CHINNANON M, CHUAYTANEE N, ANGAJCHARIYA A & SUJARIT C. 2023. Human disturbance affects behavioural time allocation in a fiddler crab (Austruca annulipes) in Southern Thailand. J Anim Behav Biometeorol 11(3): 2023025.) also detected that human disturbance affected time allocation, particularly in anti-predatory mode. Given our study was conducted during winter break, a high tourist season, increased trampling is likely to be happening in response to the higher human density. Therefore, the alteration in behavioral patterns of fiddler crabs from the HAI population could have the same explanations as these populations from Korea and Thailand.

The third possible explanation concerns past selective pressures. Such pressures – anthropogenic or not – at the individual level may have resulted in a population with individuals more responsive to humans (Bejder et al. 2009BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, FINN H & ALLEN S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177-185.). Thus, the HAI population may be responding to human presence not due to sensitization but due to historical pressures generated by tourism and urbanization, including exposure to new chemicals (sunscreen or aerosols); water and air pollution; and higher incidence of light (by artificial means or removal of vegetation) (Sadoul et al. 2021SADOUL B, BLUMSTEIN DT, ALFONSO S & GEFFROY B. 2021. Human protection drives the emergence of a new coping style in animals. PLoS Biol 19: e3001186.).

Limitations of the study

There are certain limitations to the universalization of the results obtained by the research. First, habituation and sensitization take time to observe (Bejder et al. 2009BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, FINN H & ALLEN S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177-185.), and effects of humans on crab activity can be detected a few days later (DiNuzzo et al. 2020DINUZZO ER, ANDERSON L III, WALKER A, SASSO HK, CHRISTENSEN B & GRIFFEN BD. 2020. Human influences on male waving behavior in the fiddler crab Leptuca pugilator. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol 53: 43-57.) and a prolonged follow-up of these populations is needed. Second, although we analysed 80 individuals, they belonged to only two beaches inside a single mangrove. Therefore, including replicas from other mangroves and beaches would add more robustness to the results observed here, since some effects of anthropization, like trampling, can last six weeks (Park & Kim 2021PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178.). Third, a more detailed environmental survey can elucidate the effects of other environmental variables, obtaining a more holistic understanding of the phenomenon observed here. Some of these variables are the percentage of substrate moisture around the burrow and inside the burrow; substrate granulometry; biomass adhered to the substrate; chemical composition and physical properties of water in or around their burrows; incidence of predators; and the average size of individuals (Checon & Costa 2017CHECON HH & COSTA TM. 2017. Fiddler crab (Crustacea: Ocypodidae) distribution and the relationship between habitat occupancy and mouth appendages. Mar Biol Res 13: 618-629.), variables that influence the distribution of species, but little is known if there is any influence at the individual level (exceptions include (Hewes & Chaves-Campos 2018HEWES ME & CHAVES-CAMPOS J. 2018. Boldness related to size in the hermit crabCoenobita compressusat undisturbed, but not disturbed beach. Ethology 124: 570-578., Chen et al. 2019CHEN T-Y, HWANG G-W, MAYFIELD AB, CHEN C-P & LIN H-J. 2019. The development of habitat suitability models for fiddler crabs residing in subtropical tidal flats. Ocean Coast Manag 182: 104931.).

Our results emphasize the contrasting behavioral patterns of these two populations, where the LAI population fed for longer after the stimulus and showed a less intense response than the HAI population. Given that each population experiences distinct levels of interaction with humans, such interaction may influence not only these animals’ escape behavior but also have cascade effects, with less time dedicated to foraging. However, since recording time is limited, our study does not represent the full behavioral repertoire of fiddler crabs, let alone the full extent of these effects. It is noteworthy that foraging behaviors are essential for animals as they are directly linked to caloric intake, and a calorie deficit can compromise health and reproduction (Navedo et al. 2019NAVEDO JG, VERDUGO C, RODRÍGUEZ-JORQUERA IA, ABAD-GÓMEZ JM, SUAZO CG, CASTAÑEDA LE, ARAYA V, RUIZ J & GUTIÉRREZ JS. 2019. Assessing the effects of human activities on the foraging opportunities of migratory shorebirds in Austral high-latitude bays. PLoS ONE 14: e0212441., Park & Kim 2021PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178.). Habituation to humans is considered a negative result of interaction with wildlife since these interactions pose several risks to animals and their populations such as dependence on human food (change in activity budget), attacks on humans and disease transmission (Orams 2002ORAMS MB. 2002. Feeding wildlife as a tourism attraction: a review of issues and impacts. Tourism Manage 23: 281-293., Geist 2011GEIST V. 2011. advances in understanding and management application. Hum-wildl interact 5: 9-12.). Sensitization has been proposed as a mitigating strategy to reduce human wildlife conflict (Honda et al. 2019HONDA T, YAMABATA N, IIJIMA H & UCHIDA K. 2019. Sensitization to human decreases human-wildlife conflict: empirical and simulation study. Eur J Wildl Res 65.). However, it can be costly as risk is a constant aspect of life (Blumstein 2016BLUMSTEIN DT. 2016. Habituation and sensitization: new thoughts about old ideas. Anim Behav 120: 255-262.). The influence of human presence on wildlife, even if peaceful (as is the case with tourism), is the subject of intense research, with several consequences to wildlife such as territorial area, foraging behavior, and reproductive success (Gander & Ingold 1997GANDER H & INGOLD P. 1997. Reactions of male alpine chamois Rupicapra r. rupicapra to hikers, joggers and mountainbikers. Biol Conserv 79: 107-109., Bejder et al. 2006BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, GALES N, MANN J, CONNOR R, HEITHAUS M, WATSON-CAPPS J, FLAHERTY C & KRÜTZEN M. 2006. Decline in relative abundance of bottlenose dolphins exposed to long-term disturbance. Conserv Biol 20: 1791-1798., Christiansen & Lusseau 2013CHRISTIANSEN F & LUSSEAU D. 2013. Understanding the ecological effects of whale-watching on cetaceans. In: WILLIAMS JHLB (Ed), Whale-watching, sustainable tourism and ecological management, Cambridge University Press, p. 177-192.).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

A preliminary version of this paper was presented to a committee in partial fulfilment of the requirements for a Degree in Biology at Universidade Estadual Paulista. We thank the A. P. Ferreira, A. L. Arvigo and G. B. O. Machado for their contributions for species identification. We thank F. S. Búfalo for comments on early versions of this manuscript. LC receives a Research Productivity Fellowship from the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico - CNPq (#314964/2021-5).

REFERENCES

  • ABRAHAMS MI, PERES CA & COSTA HCM. 2018. Manioc losses by terrestrial vertebrates in western Brazilian Amazonia. J Wildl Manage 82: 734-746.
  • ALONGI DM. 2015. The impact of climate change on mangrove forests. Curr Clim Change Rep 1: 30-39.
  • ANGELINI C, VAN MONTFRANS SG, HENSEL MJS, HE Q & SILLIMAN BR. 2018. The importance of an underestimated grazer under climate change: how crab density, consumer competition, and physical stress affect salt marsh resilience. Oecologia 187: 205-217.
  • ASENSIO-MONTESINOS F, ANFUSO G, RANDERSON P & WILLIAMS AT. 2019. Seasonal comparison of beach litter on Mediterranean coastal sites (Alicante, SE Spain). Ocean Coast Manag 181: 104914.
  • BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, FINN H & ALLEN S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177-185.
  • BEJDER L, SAMUELS A, WHITEHEAD H, GALES N, MANN J, CONNOR R, HEITHAUS M, WATSON-CAPPS J, FLAHERTY C & KRÜTZEN M. 2006. Decline in relative abundance of bottlenose dolphins exposed to long-term disturbance. Conserv Biol 20: 1791-1798.
  • BELLWOOD O. 2002. The occurrence, mechanics and significance of burying behaviour in crabs (Crustacea: Brachyura). J Nat Hist 36: 1223-1238.
  • BLUMSTEIN DT. 2016. Habituation and sensitization: new thoughts about old ideas. Anim Behav 120: 255-262.
  • CHECON HH & COSTA TM. 2017. Fiddler crab (Crustacea: Ocypodidae) distribution and the relationship between habitat occupancy and mouth appendages. Mar Biol Res 13: 618-629.
  • CHEN T-Y, HWANG G-W, MAYFIELD AB, CHEN C-P & LIN H-J. 2019. The development of habitat suitability models for fiddler crabs residing in subtropical tidal flats. Ocean Coast Manag 182: 104931.
  • CHUMSRI A, TINA FW, VONGKOK A, CHINNANON M, CHUAYTANEE N, ANGAJCHARIYA A & SUJARIT C. 2023. Human disturbance affects behavioural time allocation in a fiddler crab (Austruca annulipes) in Southern Thailand. J Anim Behav Biometeorol 11(3): 2023025.
  • CHRISTIANSEN F & LUSSEAU D. 2013. Understanding the ecological effects of whale-watching on cetaceans. In: WILLIAMS JHLB (Ed), Whale-watching, sustainable tourism and ecological management, Cambridge University Press, p. 177-192.
  • CRANE J. 1975. Fiddler crabs of the world. Ocypodidae: Genus Uca, New Jersey: Princepton University Press, p. 304-308.
  • DE GRANDE FR, GRANADO P, SANCHES FHC & COSTA TM. 2018. Organic matter affects fiddler crab distribution? Results from field and laboratorial trials. Estuar Coast Shelf Sci 212: 138-145.
  • DINUZZO ER, ANDERSON L III, WALKER A, SASSO HK, CHRISTENSEN B & GRIFFEN BD. 2020. Human influences on male waving behavior in the fiddler crab Leptuca pugilator. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol 53: 43-57.
  • FRIARD O & GAMBA M. 2016. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol Evol 7: 1325-1330.
  • GANDER H & INGOLD P. 1997. Reactions of male alpine chamois Rupicapra r. rupicapra to hikers, joggers and mountainbikers. Biol Conserv 79: 107-109.
  • GEIST V. 2011. advances in understanding and management application. Hum-wildl interact 5: 9-12.
  • GONZÁLEZ SA, YÁÑEZ-NAVEA K & MUÑOZ M. 2014. Effect of coastal urbanization on sandy beach coleoptera Phaleria maculata (Kulzer, 1959) in northern Chile. Mar Pollut Bull 83: 265-274.
  • HAMER AJ & MCDONNELL MJ. 2009. The response of herpetofauna to urbanization: Inferring patterns of persistence from wildlife databases. Austral Ecol 35: 568-580.
  • HEMMI JM. 2005a. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 1. Escape decisions in relation to the risk of predation. Anim Behav 69: 603-614.
  • HEMMI JM. 2005b. Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 2. The visual cues. Anim Behav 69: 615-625.
  • HEMMI JM & MERKLE T. 2009. High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc Biol Sci 276: 4381-4388.
  • HEMMI JM & ZEIL J. 2003. Burrow surveillance in fiddler crabs. II. The sensory cues. J Exp Biol 206: 3951-3961.
  • HEWES ME & CHAVES-CAMPOS J. 2018. Boldness related to size in the hermit crabCoenobita compressusat undisturbed, but not disturbed beach. Ethology 124: 570-578.
  • HONDA T, YAMABATA N, IIJIMA H & UCHIDA K. 2019. Sensitization to human decreases human-wildlife conflict: empirical and simulation study. Eur J Wildl Res 65.
  • JENNIONS MD, BACKWELL PRY, MURAI M & CHRISTY JH. 2003. Hiding behaviour in fiddler crabs: how long should prey hide in response to a potential predator? Anim Behav 66: 251-257.
  • KIM TW & CHOE JC. 2003. The effect of food availability on the semilunar courtship rhythm in the fiddler crab Uca lactea (de Haan) (Brachyura: Ocypodidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 54: 210-217.
  • KIM TW, KIM S & LEE JA. 2018. Effects of mudflat trampling on activity of intertidal crabs. Ocean Science Journal 53: 101-106.
  • KIM TW, SAKAMOTO K, HENMI Y & CHOE JC. 2008. To court or not to court: reproductive decisions by male fiddler crabs in response to fluctuating food availability. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62: 1139-1147.
  • KNIGHT J. 2009. Making Wildlife Viewable: Habituation and Attraction. Soc Anim 17: 167-184.
  • LECLERC M, ZEDROSSER A & PELLETIER F. 2017. Harvesting as a potential selective pressure on behavioural traits. J Appl Ecol 54: 1941-1945.
  • LEONARDI N, CARNACINA I, DONATELLI C, GANJU NK, PLATER AJ, SCHUERCH M & TEMMERMAN S. 2018. Dynamic interactions between coastal storms and salt marshes: A review. Geomorphology (Amst) 301: 92-107.
  • LOWRY H, LILL A & WONG BBM. 2013. Behavioural responses of wildlife to urban environments. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 88: 537-549.
  • MACFARLANE GR & KING SA. 2002. Observer presence influences behaviour of the semaphore crab, Heloecious cordiformis. Anim Behav 63: 1191-1194.
  • MCKINNEY ML. 2006. Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homogenization. Biol Conserv 127: 247-260.
  • DE MELO GAS. 1996. Manual de identificação dos brachyura (caranguejos e siris) do litoral brasileiro, Editora Plêiade, 603 p.
  • MORGAN D. 2018. Counting Beach Visitors: Tools, Methods and Management Applications. In: BOTERO CM et al. (Eds), Beach Management Tools - Concepts, Methodologies and Case Studies, Cham: Springer International Publishing, p. 561-577.
  • NAVEDO JG, VERDUGO C, RODRÍGUEZ-JORQUERA IA, ABAD-GÓMEZ JM, SUAZO CG, CASTAÑEDA LE, ARAYA V, RUIZ J & GUTIÉRREZ JS. 2019. Assessing the effects of human activities on the foraging opportunities of migratory shorebirds in Austral high-latitude bays. PLoS ONE 14: e0212441.
  • ORAMS MB. 2002. Feeding wildlife as a tourism attraction: a review of issues and impacts. Tourism Manage 23: 281-293.
  • PARK S & KIM TW. 2021. Human trampling decreases surface activity and disturbs behavioral rhythm of an endangered intertidal crab. Ecol Indic 131: 108178.
  • PEEKE H. 1984. Habituation, Sensitization, and Behavior, Elsevier, 486 p.
  • POLETTO & BATISTA. 2008. Sensibilidade ambiental das ilhas costeiras de Ubatuba, SP, Brasil. Ambiente & Água-An Interdisciplinary Journal 3: 106-121.
  • R DEVELOPMENT CORE TEAM. 2020. R: A language and environment for statisticalcomputing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/
    » https://www.R-project.org/
  • RADERSCHALL CA, MAGRATH RD & HEMMI JM. 2011. Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214: 4209-4216.
  • RANKIN ET AL. 2009. Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92: 135-138.
  • ROSENBERG MS. 2019. A fresh look at the biodiversity lexicon for fiddler crabs (Decapoda: Brachyura: Ocypodidae). Part 1: Taxonomy. J Crustacean Biol: 729-738.
  • SADOUL B, BLUMSTEIN DT, ALFONSO S & GEFFROY B. 2021. Human protection drives the emergence of a new coping style in animals. PLoS Biol 19: e3001186.
  • SCHNEIDER CA, RASBAND WS & ELICEIRI KW. 2012. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods 9: 671-675.
  • SIH A, FERRARI MCO & HARRIS DJ. 2011. Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evol Appl 4: 367-387.
  • SOL D, LAPIEDRA O & GONZÁLEZ-LAGOS C. 2013. Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Anim Behav 85: 1101-1112.
  • THURMAN CL, FARIA SC & MCNAMARA JC. 2013. The distribution of fiddler crabs (Uca) along the coast of Brazil: implications for biogeography of the western Atlantic Ocean. Mar Biodivers Rec 6: e1.
  • TOMSIC D, MASSONI V & MALDONADO H. 1993. Habituation to a danger stimulus in two semiterrestrial crabs: ontogenic, ecological and opioid modulation correlates. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 173.
  • TOMSIC D, PEDREIRA ME, ROMANO A, HERMITTE G & MALDONADO H. 1998. Context-us association as a determinant of long-term habituation in the crabChasmagnathus. Anim Learn Behav 26: 196-209.
  • VAN HIMBEECK RAF, HUIZINGA W, ROESSINK I & PEETERS ETHM. 2019. Behavioral patterns of two fiddler crab species Uca rapax and Uca tangeri in a seminatural mangrove system. Zoo Biol 38: 343-354.
  • VINCENT AES & HOELLEIN TJ. 2017. Anthropogenic litter abundance and accumulation rates point to seasonal litter sources on a Great Lakes Beach. Journal of Contemporary Water 160: 72-84.
  • WALKER I. 1972. Habituation to disturbance in the fiddler crab (Uca annulipes) in its natural environment. Anim Behav 20: 139-146.
  • WALTON BJ, FINDLAY LJ & HILL RA. 2022. Camera traps and guard observations as an alternative to researcher observation for studying anthropogenic foraging. Ecol Evol 12: e8808.
  • WEIS JS & WEIS P. 2004. Behavior of Four Species of Fiddler Crabs, Genus Uca, in Southeast Sulawesi, Indonesia. Hydrobiologia 523: 47-58.
  • ZEIL J & HEMMI JM. 2006. The visual ecology of fiddler crabs. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 192: 1-25.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    24 June 2024
  • Date of issue
    2024

History

  • Received
    26 June 2023
  • Accepted
    07 Apr 2024
Academia Brasileira de Ciências Rua Anfilófio de Carvalho, 29, 3º andar, 20030-060 Rio de Janeiro RJ Brasil, Tel: +55 21 3907-8100 - Rio de Janeiro - RJ - Brazil
E-mail: aabc@abc.org.br