Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The impact of a dysfluency environment on the temporal organization of consonants in stuttering

Abstracts

Purpose

To analyze and compare the voice onset time (VOT) in Brazilian Portuguese speakers who stutter and those who do not, focusing on three different moments of speech: fluent, pre-dysfluent and post-dysfluent environments.

Methods

Twenty participants (n=10 with stuttering and n=10 without stuttering) were recorded. The data were transcribed and segmented for acoustic analysis, and it was extracted tokens of Brazilian Portuguese voiceless stops /p/, /t/ and /k/. Tokens were classified according to whether they were produced by people who stutter (PWS) or by people who do not stutter (PWnS), and according to their environment in speech (i.e., in fluent speech, pre-dysfluency, and post-dysfluency). For comparisons within groups it were used the Friedman and Wilcoxon tests, and the Mann-Whitney test was used in intergroup comparisons. Statistical analyses were executed using SPSS 14.0 with the significance level set at α=0.05.

Results

VOT in stuttering and non-stuttering speakers differed most in the environment of pre-dysfluencies, during which stuttering speakers show significantly longer VOT than speakers who do not stutter. After passing through a moment of dysfluency, however, stuttering speakers’ VOT returns to measures similar to non-stuttering speakers’.

Conclusion

In pre-dysfluent and post-dysfluent speech, PWS produces longer VOT than PWnS. In the fluent speech of PWS, the stops behave differently. The implications of these results for speech motor control are discussed.

Stuttering; Acoustics; Planning; Motor skills; Speech


Objetivo

Analisar e comparar o voice onset time (VOT) em falantes do português brasileiro com e sem gagueira, com foco em três momentos diferentes de discurso: fala fluente, pré-disfluência e pós-disfluência.

Métodos

Foram feitas gravações da fala de 20 participantes (n=10 com gagueira e n=10 sem gagueira). Os dados foram transcritos e segmentados para análise acústica e segmentos de oclusivas não vozeadas do Português Brasileiro, /p/, /t/ e /k/ foram extraídos. Os segmentos foram classificados por grupo - se foram produzidos por pessoas que gaguejam (PG) ou por pessoas que não gaguejam (PnG) - e de acordo com o contexto/ambiente de fala (ou seja, na fala fluente, na pré-disfluência, e na pós-disfluência). Os testes Friedman e Wilcoxon foram utilizados para comparação dentro dos grupos e o teste de Mann-Whitney, em comparações intergrupos. As análises estatísticas foram executadas usando SPSS 14.0, com nível de significância de α=0,05.

Resultados

O VOT de falantes com e sem gagueira diferiu mais no ambiente de pré-disfluência, durante o qual, falantes com gagueira apresentaram VOT significativamente mais longo do que falantes que não gaguejavam. Depois de passar por um momento de disfluência, no entanto, o VOT dos participantes com gagueira retornou a medidas semelhantes às pessoas que não gaguejavam.

Conclusão

Nos ambientes de pré-disfluência e pós-disfluência, as PG produzem VOT mais longos do que as PnG. Já no discurso fluente de PG, as oclusivas se comportam de forma diferente. São discutidas as implicações desses resultados para o controle motor da fala.

Gagueira; Acústica; Planejamento; Destreza motora; Fala


INTRODUCTION

Acoustically, the production of stop consonants involves three stages: (1) the gap, (2) the release burst, and (3) the interval between the burst and the beginning of phonation. The interval between the beginning of the burst and the beginning of phonation is called voice onset time, or VOT(1Hertrich I, Ackermann PH. Exploration of orofacial speech movements. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p. 132-9.,2Lousada ML. Estudo da produção de oclusivas do português europeu [dissertação]. Aveiro: Secção Autônoma de Ciências da Saúde; 2006.). Fluent speech requires speakers to efficiently coordinate oral-facial muscles and the vibration of vocal folds, and these skills are often lacking in people who stutter (PWS)(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.). Such incoordination can result in speech disruptions, compromising articulatory stability. This phenomenon may be observed in acoustic measurements, and through further examination of stops in context, VOT measures may shed further light on articulatory instability in PWS.

Generally, speech production requires three phases: elaboration, preparation, and execution. These phases must be accurately controlled, and their temporal basis coordinated(4Pinto S. Bases anatomophysiologiques de l’articulation supralaryngée. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p 53-6.). Each articulatory speech gesture can be seen as a ‘coordinated structure’ of different factors(1Hertrich I, Ackermann PH. Exploration of orofacial speech movements. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p. 132-9.), based on either theories which support exclusive central control or theories which support speech as a dynamic task of coordinated articulatory movements(4Pinto S. Bases anatomophysiologiques de l’articulation supralaryngée. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p 53-6.). Each sound involves the coordination of specific muscular movements, which must be modified over a period.

In order to produce the stop consonants, it is initially necessary for a speaker to decide whether the vocal folds will participate — that is, whether “voicing” will be involved.

Independently of the vocal folds vibration, the palate must be raised so that the nasal tract is blocked and the air is prevented from escaping through the oral cavity(1Hertrich I, Ackermann PH. Exploration of orofacial speech movements. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p. 132-9.), generating a build-up of air and increasing pressure in the mouth. Then, the articulators involved release the airstream(5Zemlin WR. Princípios de anatomia e fisiologia em fonoaudiologia. 4a ed. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas Sul; 2000.).

The definition of which articulators are involved is important, as there is a relation between the place of articulation and the duration of the intra-oral air pressure release(6Abdelli-Beruh NB. Influence of place of articulation on some acoustic correlates of the stop voicing contrast in Parisian French. J Phonetics. 2009;37:66-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.09.0...
). In a study of several languages(7Lisker L, Abramson AS. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word. 1964;20(3):384-422.) the authors showed that velar stops present a higher VOT than anterior stops. The complexity of the articulatory movements involved in the production of stop consonants confirms the need for studies of people who have communication disorders.

Because of the articulatory complexity of stops, VOT has been studied in communication disorders such as dyslexia(8Giraud K, Trébuchon-DaFonseca A, Démonet JF, Habib M, Liégeois-Chauvel C. Asymmetry of voice onset time-processing in adult developmental dyslexics. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(7):1652-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.02.017
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.02...
), stuttering(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.,9Max L, Gracco VL. Coordination of oral and laryngeal movements in the perceptually fluent speech of adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res.2005;48(3):524-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/036)
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/0...
), aphasia and dysarthria(1010 Auzou P, Özsancak C, Morris RJ, Jan M, Eustache F, Hannequin D. Voice onset time in aphasia, apraxia of speech and dysarthria: a review. Clin Ling Phonet. 2000;14(2):131-50.), among others. Recent studies suggest that basal ganglia have an important role in speech timing(4Pinto S. Bases anatomophysiologiques de l’articulation supralaryngée. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p 53-6.), and when these are affected by disorders, speakers may show great variation in the duration of stops. A study of individuals with Parkinson’s disease showed that these patients tend to mix VOT patterns, for example by strengthening the front stops, because the more forward the stops are, the shorter is the VOT(7Lisker L, Abramson AS. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word. 1964;20(3):384-422.). On researching the relationship between Huntington’s chorea and voiceless stops(1Hertrich I, Ackermann PH. Exploration of orofacial speech movements. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p. 132-9.), there was a decrease in the VOT mean over time. Researches also suggest that stuttering may be related to a dysfunction of the basal ganglia(1111 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.0...

12 Alm PA. Stuttering and sensory gating: a study of acoustic startle prepulse inhibition. Brain Lang. 2006;97(3):317-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12....
-1313 Smits-Bandstra S, De Nil LF. Sequence skill learning in persons who stutter: implications for cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical dysfunction. J Fluency Disord. 2007;32(4):251-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.0...
).

Stuttering is considered to be a speech fluency disorder(1414 Arcuri CF, Osborn E, Schiefer AM, Chiari BM. Taxa de elocução de fala segundo a gravidade da gagueira. Pró Fono. 2009;21(1):45-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872009000100008
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-5687200900...
) centering on difficulties with motor control in speech(1111 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.0...
-1212 Alm PA. Stuttering and sensory gating: a study of acoustic startle prepulse inhibition. Brain Lang. 2006;97(3):317-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12....
,1515 Giraud AL, Neumann K, Bachoud-Levi A, Gudenberg AW, Euler HA, Lanfermann H et al. Severity of dysfluency correlates with basal ganglia activity in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Lang. 2008;104(2):190-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04....

16 Wu JC, Maguire G, Riley G, Fallon J, LaCasse L, Chin S et al. A positron emission tomography [18F] deoxyglucose study of developmental stuttering. Neuroreport. 1995;6(3):501-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199502000-00024
https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-1995020...
-1717 Rosenberger PB. Dopaminergic systems and speech fluency. J Fluency Disord. 1980;5(3):255-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(80)90031-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(80)900...
). PWS present disruption in their speech processing ability with temporal stability(1818 Andrade CRF, Cervone LM, Sassi FC. Relationship between the stuttering severity index and speech rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2003;121(2):81-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802003000200010
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-3180200300...
), and for that reason, there have been more and more researchers studying VOT among PWS(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.,9Max L, Gracco VL. Coordination of oral and laryngeal movements in the perceptually fluent speech of adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res.2005;48(3):524-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/036)
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/0...
,1919 Arenas RM, Zebrowski PM, Moon JB. Phonetically governed voicing onset and offset in preschool children who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2012;37(3):179-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.04.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.0...
,2020 Jäncke L. Variability and duration of voice onset time and phonation in stuttering and non-stuttering adults. J Fluency Disord. 1994;19(1):21-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)90012-4
https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)900...
) , since VOT is a standard methodology for investigating temporal features of speech. A study showed(2121 Hillman RE, Gilbert HR. Voice onset time for voiceless stop consonants in the fluent reading of stutterers and nonstutterers. J Acoust Soc Amer. 1977;61(2):610-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.381308
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.381308...
) that PWS produce a longer VOT for voiceless stops, while another one(2222 Adams MR. Voice onsets and segment durations of normal speakers and beginning stutterers. J Fluency Disord. 1987;12(2):133-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(87)90019-2
https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(87)900...
) observed that stuttering children present a longer VOT for both voiced and voiceless stops when compared to non-stuttering children.

In a study with stuttering and non-stuttering native speakers of German, participants were instructed to produce isolated syllables (/papapas/, /tatatas/, and /kakakas/) with stress on the second syllable. The results showed that even in fluent speech, the stuttering participants presented a higher variation in the duration of the first syllable production(2020 Jäncke L. Variability and duration of voice onset time and phonation in stuttering and non-stuttering adults. J Fluency Disord. 1994;19(1):21-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)90012-4
https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)900...
). On a research of /p/ in sentences, analyzing the difference between the productions of stuttering and non-stuttering participants through eletroglotography and acoustic measurements, the results suggest a difference between the two groups in the duration of intervals of oral-laryngeal subsystem events(9Max L, Gracco VL. Coordination of oral and laryngeal movements in the perceptually fluent speech of adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res.2005;48(3):524-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/036)
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/0...
).

A Brazilian study compared how stutterers and non-stutterers produce stops preceded by a vowel (e.g. [ap]). The results indicated that PWS’ segments were longer, and the difference increased with the degree of stuttering(2323 Arcuri CF, Osborn E, Schiefer AM, Chiari BM. Duração do onset vocálico da fala fluente de gagos. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15(1):108-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342010000100018
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-8034201000...
).

In order to verify the use of VOT as a parameter of speech naturalness in comparing the outcomes of two stuttering treatment procedures, authors analyzed the production of /b/ in sentences uttered by stuttering and non-stuttering speakers(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.). They found VOT to be a satisfactory parameter: stutterers exhibited longer VOTs, which was interpreted as a dysfunction of the neuromotor process involved in speech(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.).

Research on VOT and its role in stuttering offers important resources for understanding articulatory processes. The present study contributes to this trend, providing an acoustic description of VOT among stuttering and non-stuttering speakers of Brazilian Portuguese considering the dysfluency environment as main tool on understanding of planning motor error. The purpose of this study is to verify how voice onset time (VOT) is produce by people who stutter (PWS) focusing on three different moments of speech: fluent, pre-dysfluent and post-dysfluent environments.

METHODS

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), resolution number CAAE – 0308.0.203.000-11. All participants signed a term of free and informed consent; according to the demands of the 196/96 act (BRAZIL. Act MS/CNS/CNEP number 196/96 of October 10th 1996).

The 20 participants in this study were divided in two different groups: 10 in the experimental group (PWS) and 10 in the control group (non-stutterers). All were males between the ages of 20 and 45, since sex and age appears to influence on VOT(2424 Celeste LC, Teixeira EG P. Efeito do sexo e idade na produção do VOT. Rev Letras. 2009;2(1):28-39.). Criteria for participation included an absence of general health deficits and negative screening results for communication disorders (language, hearing, neurologic, cognitive, etc.). All participants were screened by a speech therapist in order to ensure that selection criteria were fulfilled. All members of the experimental group presented developmental stuttering at a moderate to severe level on the scale of Iowa(2525 Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering I: persistency and recovery rates. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(5):1097-112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1097
https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1097...
), but they had no other sensorial disorders or speech or hearing troubles. None had undergone any kind of treatment to improve their speech. The control group presented normal fluency as measured by the Speech Fluency Assessment Protocol(2626 Andrade CRF. Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró Fono. 2000;12(2):131-4.).

Two sets of data were collected for the study. To elicit fluent, unrehearsed speech, following the Speech Fluency Assessment Protocol(2626 Andrade CRF. Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró Fono. 2000;12(2):131-4.), a picture was presented to each participant and the following instructions were given: “Please look at this picture and tell me anything you want about it.” Participants’ responses were audio recorded.

To elicit specific productions of all voiceless stops in Brazilian Portuguese, participants were also asked to read sample sentences aloud. Each participant was given ten sentences to read silently. The sentences featured voiceless stops /p/, /t/, and /k/ in different positions (e.g., word-initial Eu gosto da Carol ‘I like Carol’; inter-vocalic eu deixei o recado ‘I left the note’). Later, in a soundproof environment, participants were instructed to read the sentences aloud three times into a microphone connected to a Praat-enabled computer (version 5.1.02, 1992-2010, freely available on www.praat.org). The data were stored for later editing and analysis.

All speech samples were transcribed orthographically to identify fluent and dysfluent syllables, and all speech disruptions were classified (hesitation, interjection, revision, unfinished word, word repetition, segment repetition, phrase repetition, syllable repetition, sound repetition, prolongation, blocks, pauses and intrusion of sounds or segments). Syllables were also counted, excluding non-word interjections, revisions, non-finished words, word repetitions, segment repetitions, phrase repetitions, and syllable repetitions; and these totals were used to calculate speech rates and frequencies of speech disruptions.

Speech rate variation has been shown to be an important factor in VOT analysis(2121 Hillman RE, Gilbert HR. Voice onset time for voiceless stop consonants in the fluent reading of stutterers and nonstutterers. J Acoust Soc Amer. 1977;61(2):610-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.381308
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.381308...
); however, participants’ speech dysfluencies do not necessarily represent a difference in the articulation time of each syllable. For these reasons, measures of speech rate were calculated for each recorded utterance. Speech rate was calculated by dividing the number of syllables by the articulation time (duration of pauses and dysfluencies subtracted from the total duration of the utterance), resulting in the articulation rate. The articulation rate mean was 5.5 syllables/seconds. Sentences whose articulation rate had a standard deviation of higher than 1.5 were eliminated.

The transcribed data were then segmented for acoustic analysis, and tokens of the Brazilian Portuguese voiceless stops /p/, /t/ and /k/ were identified and extracted. The consonants included in the analysis occurred in different sentence positions, and were required to appear in the context VCV (vowel-consonant-vowel). Further, the analytical focus centered on syllables in unstressed position. Stressed and utterance-final syllables were excluded because they differ in duration from unstressed syllables.

Three parameters of the voiceless stops were analyzed: the silence or occlusion duration, the VOT, and the total duration. Duration of occlusion was marked between the end of the preceding vowel and the beginning of the release burst. VOT boundaries were marked from the beginning of the burst to the beginning of the following vowel (Figure 1). A triangulation of waveform, spectrogram, and F0 curve was used to determine these boundaries.

Figure 1
Boundary Identification. First tier (spelling) = transcription of the utterance; second tier (aa) = acoustic analysis of the stop consonant; third tier (B exp) = release burst

To determine the effect of dysfluency environments on voiceless stops, each stop parameter was investigated in the following speech environments:

  1. Fluency: there was no dysfluency in the word which preceded or followed the token word;

  2. Pre-dysfluency: the stop occurs in a syllable just before a dysfluency, and

  3. Post-dysfluency: the stop occurs in a syllable immediately following the dysfluency.

The number of analyzed consonants was greater for PWS than for PWnS. Several PWS asked to reread the sentences, and these additional tokens were included in the data.

Interjudge reliability measures were obtained for determining moments of disfluency in the PWS´s speech. In order to identify moments of dysfluency, a perceptive test was administered to three Brazilian native-speakers. None of them presented stuttering or any other speech, language or auditory disorder. The test was administered individually, in a silent place in each auditor’s home. The auditors were given a piece of paper listing the sentences read by PWS. While listening to the recordings, each auditor was asked to underline moments of dysfluency on the list. The volume of the recordings was adjusted individually according to each listener’s comfort level, and the sentences were played several times so that evaluators could be sure about the labeling. Only markings identified by all the three auditors and the research were included in the analysis.

The statistical analysis entailed separating the groups (PWS and PWnS) and the speech environments as follows: (a) the consonants compared to each other; (b) the consonants in PWS’s speech compared to those in PWnS’s speech; and (c) speech environments compared to each other. Descriptive and inferential measures were calculated. The Friedman and Wilcoxon tests were used for comparisons within groups, and the Mann-Whitney test was used in intergroup comparisons. Statistical analyses were executed using SPSS 14.0 with the significance level set at α=.05..

RESULTS

The effect of speech environment on consonants and their release bursts was striking. Tables 1 and 2 below show the effects of speech environment on each consonant for both the control and the experimental group. As described above, the analysis of the experimental group comprised three categories: fluency, pre-dysfluency, and post-dysfluency.

Tabela 1
Medidas descritivas das oclusivas não vozeadas produzidas por pessoas com gagueira, em todos os três ambientes experimentais e no grupo controle
Tabela 2
Comparação dos valores de p: intervalo de oclusão, VOT e duração total das consoantes para cada contexto de fala

For PWS, the acoustic measurements of occlusion time and VOT show differentiated voiceless stops in all speech environments. However, consonants did not differ by total duration measures in post-dysfluent speech. In the control group, voiceless stops differed in occlusion time and VOT measures.

An analysis of the effects of speech environment (PWnS, PWS fluent, PWS pre-dysfluent and PWS post-dysfluent) showed that most p values demonstrate a statistically significant difference. In the intergroup analysis (PWnS versus PWSs’ speech environments), PWS’s VOT was longer in five of nine comparisons and was shorter for /t/ in fluent and /p/ in pre-dysfluent environments. The acoustic measures of fluent /p/ showed no difference between PWnS and PWS. The voiceless stop occlusion times were longer for PWS in four of nine comparisons and shorter for /p/ and /k/ in post-dysfluent environments. For consonants, PWS shower higher total duration values in five of nine comparisons; however, durations of /p/ and /k/ were shorter in post-dysfluent environments (Table 3).

Table 3
Comparison of p values: groups and speech environments

Table 3 also shows a comparison of speech environments for PWS. For these speakers, VOT is usually longer in post-dysfluent speech, while occlusion time and total duration are longer in fluent speech. In pre-dysfluent speech, all acoustic measurements for voiceless stops showed higher values than in fluent speech. Tokens in pre-dysfluent environments also generally showed higher values than those in post-dysfluent speech, with the exception of VOT for /p/, which is shorter in pre-dysfluent environments.

DISCUSSION

This study verified the influence of speech environment (fluent, pre-dysfluent and post-dysfluent) on the production of voiceless stops by speakers of Brazilian Portuguese who stutter. For pre-dysfluent speech, PWS measures were higher than those for PWnS in all measurements taken for each consonant. Three possible hypotheses could justify such a difference.

Different theories identify the formulation of language as a primary factor in the production of speech dysfluencies in both fluent speakers as well as the speakers who stutter. One theory argued that dysfluencies are consequences of errors detected during the preparation of the phonetic plan(2727 Potmas A, Kolk H. The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluências. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;3:472-87.). The results found in the pre-dysfluency environment may reflect an adjustment of articulatory gestures to identifying errors in phonetic planning, corroborating all of these ideas.

In neuromotor terms, slower articulation in PWS can be interpreted as a dysfunction in speech processing(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.). The hypothesis that stuttering was the result of a dysfunction of the basal ganglia reinforces the idea that the disturbance is directly related to timing in speech production(1111 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.0...
,1212 Alm PA. Stuttering and sensory gating: a study of acoustic startle prepulse inhibition. Brain Lang. 2006;97(3):317-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12....
). This dysfunction of the basal ganglia result of a structural abnormality that affects the flow of information between Broca’s area and the motor cortex, ie, between the programming of speech motor planning and execution of movement(1515 Giraud AL, Neumann K, Bachoud-Levi A, Gudenberg AW, Euler HA, Lanfermann H et al. Severity of dysfluency correlates with basal ganglia activity in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Lang. 2008;104(2):190-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04....
).

A third hypothesis that may account for these results is known as the coarticulatory effect. The phenomenon of coarticulation has been a central theme in current studies on speech production and can be generally defined as the overlapping of sounds during speech production. This means that the production of sound /b/, for example, is different when produced alone or in syllables /bu/ or /bi/: in the former, /b/ is accompanied by an anticipatory rounding of the lips, while the latter is accompanied by stretched lips. This is just one example of coarticulation in Brazilian Portuguese.

It is to be expected, then, that speech shows wide variability; and speech signal segmentation is difficult given the continuous and reciprocal influence of speech segments, a fact that is virtually universal(2828 Tatham M, Morton K. Speech production and perception. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2006.). Some studies have produced the noteworthy finding that coarticulation appears to have similar or identical influences on the speech of those who stutter and those who do not(2929 Sussman HM, Byrd CT, Guitar B. The integrity of anticipatory coarticulation in fluent and non-fluent tokens of adults who stutter. Clin Linguist Phon. 2011;25(3):169-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2010.517896
https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2010.51...
), that is, the effect of coarticulation is present in the speech of PWS. Furthermore, coarticulation can be subdivided into two groups: left-to-right (LR) and right-to-left (RL). The first group, LR, is also called “progressive,” because certain properties of one segment are retained and extend into the next segment. The second group, RL, is also called “regressive.” Its coarticulation is related to anticipatory effects; that is, one segment influences those that come before it(2828 Tatham M, Morton K. Speech production and perception. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2006.). The RL, more common in spontaneous speech, can be explained by cognitive intervention in biological and biomechanical processes(2828 Tatham M, Morton K. Speech production and perception. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2006.).

A decrease in articulation speed is expected during dysfluency. In the present study, we evaluated the effect of dysfluency on consonant syllables in pre- and post-dysfluency environments. Findings corroborated that regressive coarticulation is more common than progressive. The effects of dysfluencies were much more striking in pre-dysfluent than in post-dysfluent environments. This fact is apparent in Tables 1, 2 and 3, where the PWS VOT is greater than the PWnS, but the occlusion time tends to be lower in the group with stuttering. Thus, the effects of dysfluencies seem to be bidirectional, predominantly in pre-dysfluent environments.

The variability of phone behavior in fluent PWS speech also supports the findings of a study(2020 Jäncke L. Variability and duration of voice onset time and phonation in stuttering and non-stuttering adults. J Fluency Disord. 1994;19(1):21-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)90012-4
https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)900...
) that demonstrated greater variability in the measures of voiceless occlusive duration among speakers who stutter. However, our study does not corroborate other findings(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.,2121 Hillman RE, Gilbert HR. Voice onset time for voiceless stop consonants in the fluent reading of stutterers and nonstutterers. J Acoust Soc Amer. 1977;61(2):610-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.381308
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.381308...
) because the results on VOT duration for the bilabial phoneme /p/ were virtually the same for speakers who stutter and those who do not, while they were different in the studies cited above. The difference in findings can be explained by differences in the speech environments analyzed. Here, we separate speech dysfluency into three different environmental moments, while those researches(3Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.,2121 Hillman RE, Gilbert HR. Voice onset time for voiceless stop consonants in the fluent reading of stutterers and nonstutterers. J Acoust Soc Amer. 1977;61(2):610-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.381308
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.381308...
) apparently considered only fluent speech.

It is important to highlight that for the production of fluent /k/, the occlusion time of PWS is much greater than that of fluent speakers (PWnS), offsetting the reduced VOT in determining the duration of the phone. This offers further evidence of the great variability and instability in motor control among individuals who stutter(1111 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.0...
,1212 Alm PA. Stuttering and sensory gating: a study of acoustic startle prepulse inhibition. Brain Lang. 2006;97(3):317-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12....
).

Many studies have shown that changes in the duration of VOT follow a hierarchical order for stops: velar> alveolar> labial(1Hertrich I, Ackermann PH. Exploration of orofacial speech movements. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p. 132-9.,2Lousada ML. Estudo da produção de oclusivas do português europeu [dissertação]. Aveiro: Secção Autônoma de Ciências da Saúde; 2006.). This study expected that PWS and PWnS would maintain this trend, albeit with higher values for the first group. For PWnS, this relationship remains clear, with statistically significant differences between the consonants. However, our data for PWS did not confirmed this assumption. These results therefore corroborate those reported on a research of temporal organization in speech disorders, in such the duration of the consonants uttered by individuals who stutter do not follow the typical trends determined by location and manner of articulation(3030 Cardoso BAS, Reis C. Variables for the study of the temporal organization in speech disorders. In: Proceedings of IV Speech Prosody; 6-9 maio 2008; Campinas, Brasil. [Local, editor e data desconhecidos] p. 195-8.).

CONCLUSION

In pre-dysfluent speech, the stuttering group showed higher durations for all measurements (total duration, VOT, and occlusive duration) taken for each voiceless stop.

Considering that none of the subjects in this study had participated in speech therapy, the results encourage further investigation into whether speech therapy might affect the temporal consonant features described here. We wish to take a conservative approach in interpreting our data, and we acknowledge that the general degree of stuttering among PWS in this study was relatively severe. Future work will need to consider different levels of stuttering severity. In addition, it may be interesting to apply a similar methodology to samples of spontaneous speech.

Table 1
Descriptive measures of voiceless stops produced by people people who stutter (PWS) in all three experimental environments and in the control group (PWnS)
Table 2
Comparison of p values: occlusion time, VOT, and consonants’ total duration for each speech environment

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Hertrich I, Ackermann PH. Exploration of orofacial speech movements. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p. 132-9.
  • 2
    Lousada ML. Estudo da produção de oclusivas do português europeu [dissertação]. Aveiro: Secção Autônoma de Ciências da Saúde; 2006.
  • 3
    Sassi FC, Andrade FC. Acoustic analyses of speech naturalness: a comparison between two therapeutic approaches. Pró Fono. 2004;16(1):31-8.
  • 4
    Pinto S. Bases anatomophysiologiques de l’articulation supralaryngée. In: Auzou P, Rolland V, Pinto S, Ozsancak C. Les dysarthries. Marseille: Solal; 2007. p 53-6.
  • 5
    Zemlin WR. Princípios de anatomia e fisiologia em fonoaudiologia. 4a ed. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas Sul; 2000.
  • 6
    Abdelli-Beruh NB. Influence of place of articulation on some acoustic correlates of the stop voicing contrast in Parisian French. J Phonetics. 2009;37:66-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.09.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.09.002
  • 7
    Lisker L, Abramson AS. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word. 1964;20(3):384-422.
  • 8
    Giraud K, Trébuchon-DaFonseca A, Démonet JF, Habib M, Liégeois-Chauvel C. Asymmetry of voice onset time-processing in adult developmental dyslexics. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(7):1652-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.02.017
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.02.017
  • 9
    Max L, Gracco VL. Coordination of oral and laryngeal movements in the perceptually fluent speech of adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res.2005;48(3):524-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/036)
    » https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/036)
  • 10
    Auzou P, Özsancak C, Morris RJ, Jan M, Eustache F, Hannequin D. Voice onset time in aphasia, apraxia of speech and dysarthria: a review. Clin Ling Phonet. 2000;14(2):131-50.
  • 11
    Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
  • 12
    Alm PA. Stuttering and sensory gating: a study of acoustic startle prepulse inhibition. Brain Lang. 2006;97(3):317-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.001
  • 13
    Smits-Bandstra S, De Nil LF. Sequence skill learning in persons who stutter: implications for cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical dysfunction. J Fluency Disord. 2007;32(4):251-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.06.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.06.001
  • 14
    Arcuri CF, Osborn E, Schiefer AM, Chiari BM. Taxa de elocução de fala segundo a gravidade da gagueira. Pró Fono. 2009;21(1):45-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872009000100008
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872009000100008
  • 15
    Giraud AL, Neumann K, Bachoud-Levi A, Gudenberg AW, Euler HA, Lanfermann H et al. Severity of dysfluency correlates with basal ganglia activity in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Lang. 2008;104(2):190-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.04.005
  • 16
    Wu JC, Maguire G, Riley G, Fallon J, LaCasse L, Chin S et al. A positron emission tomography [18F] deoxyglucose study of developmental stuttering. Neuroreport. 1995;6(3):501-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199502000-00024
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199502000-00024
  • 17
    Rosenberger PB. Dopaminergic systems and speech fluency. J Fluency Disord. 1980;5(3):255-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(80)90031-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(80)90031-5
  • 18
    Andrade CRF, Cervone LM, Sassi FC. Relationship between the stuttering severity index and speech rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2003;121(2):81-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802003000200010
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802003000200010
  • 19
    Arenas RM, Zebrowski PM, Moon JB. Phonetically governed voicing onset and offset in preschool children who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2012;37(3):179-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.04.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.04.001
  • 20
    Jäncke L. Variability and duration of voice onset time and phonation in stuttering and non-stuttering adults. J Fluency Disord. 1994;19(1):21-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)90012-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(94)90012-4
  • 21
    Hillman RE, Gilbert HR. Voice onset time for voiceless stop consonants in the fluent reading of stutterers and nonstutterers. J Acoust Soc Amer. 1977;61(2):610-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.381308
    » https://doi.org/10.1121/1.381308
  • 22
    Adams MR. Voice onsets and segment durations of normal speakers and beginning stutterers. J Fluency Disord. 1987;12(2):133-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(87)90019-2
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(87)90019-2
  • 23
    Arcuri CF, Osborn E, Schiefer AM, Chiari BM. Duração do onset vocálico da fala fluente de gagos. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15(1):108-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342010000100018
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342010000100018
  • 24
    Celeste LC, Teixeira EG P. Efeito do sexo e idade na produção do VOT. Rev Letras. 2009;2(1):28-39.
  • 25
    Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering I: persistency and recovery rates. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(5):1097-112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1097
    » https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1097
  • 26
    Andrade CRF. Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró Fono. 2000;12(2):131-4.
  • 27
    Potmas A, Kolk H. The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluências. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;3:472-87.
  • 28
    Tatham M, Morton K. Speech production and perception. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2006.
  • 29
    Sussman HM, Byrd CT, Guitar B. The integrity of anticipatory coarticulation in fluent and non-fluent tokens of adults who stutter. Clin Linguist Phon. 2011;25(3):169-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2010.517896
    » https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2010.517896
  • 30
    Cardoso BAS, Reis C. Variables for the study of the temporal organization in speech disorders. In: Proceedings of IV Speech Prosody; 6-9 maio 2008; Campinas, Brasil. [Local, editor e data desconhecidos] p. 195-8.
  • Funding: Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES).
  • Study conducted at the Phonetic Laboratory, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais – UFMG – Belo Horizonte (MG), Brazil.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Mar 2015

History

  • Received
    3 Sept 2014
  • Accepted
    9 Mar 2015
Academia Brasileira de Audiologia Rua Itapeva, 202, conjunto 61, CEP 01332-000, Tel.: (11) 3253-8711, Fax: (11) 3253-8473 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@audiologiabrasil.org.br