Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter

ABSTRACT

Purpose

The aim of this study was to compare the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter during spontaneous speech, automatic speech, and singing.

Methods

The study sample was composed of 34 adults, 17 individuals who stutter and 17 fluent controls, matched for gender and age. The speech performance of participants was compared by means of three tasks: monologue, automatic speech, and singing. The following aspects were assessed: total number of common disruptions and total number of stuttering-like disruptions.

Results

Statistically significant difference was observed only for the monologue task in both intra- and inter-group comparisons.

Conclusion

The outcomes of this study indicate that tasks of higher motor and melodic complexities, such as the monologue task, negatively affect the speech fluency of both fluent speakers and individuals who stutter.

Keywords
Language; Speech Disorders; Stuttering

RESUMO

Objetivo

O objetivo do estudo foi comparar a performance de fala do indivíduo com gagueira e do indivíduo fluente em tarefa de fala espontânea, tarefa de fala automática e a tarefa de canto.

Método

Participaram deste estudo 34 adultos, 17 com gagueira e 17 fluentes, pareados por gênero e idade. O estudo comparou o desempenho dos participantes em três tarefas de fala: monólogo, fala automática e canto. Foi analisado o número total de rupturas comuns e gagas.

Resultados

A tarefa de monólogo foi a única que apresentou diferenças estatisticamente significativas, tanto nas comparações intragrupos quanto nas comparações intergrupos.

Conclusão

O estudo mostrou que tarefas de maior complexidade motora e melódica, como a tarefa de monólogo, prejudica a fluência da fala, tanto em indivíduos com gagueira quanto em indivíduos fluentes.

Descritores
Linguagem; Distúrbios da Fala; Gagueira

INTRODUCTION

Developmental stuttering is characterized by the occurrence of involuntary interruptions in speech motion behaviors, such as block, repetitions of sounds and syllables, prolongations, long pauses, and broken words. These alterations slow the speech rate and cause a degree of disruption above the rate pertinent to the age of the speaker(11 Bloodstein O. Incipient and developed stuttering as two distinct disorders: resolving a dilemma. J Fluency Disord. 2001;26(1):67-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0094-730X(00)00077-2.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0094-730X(00)...
). A recent study concluded, based on epidemiological analyses, that the incidence of stuttering may be greater than 5%, whereas its prevalence may be less than 1%(22 Yairi E, Ambrose N. Epidemiology of Stuttering: 21st Century Advances. J Fluency Disord. 2013;38(2):66-87. PMid:23773662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.11.002.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012...
). However, it is not yet known which mechanisms trigger speech disruptions in people who stutter(33 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. PMid:15159193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004...

4 Packman A, Code C, Onslow M. On the cause of stuttering: integrating theory with brain and behavioral research. J Neurolinguist. 2007;20(5):353-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.11.001.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2...
-55 Perez HR, Stoeckle JH. Stuttering: clinical and research update. Can Fam Physician. 2016;62(6):479-84. PMid:27303004.).

The Internal Model for Sensorimotor Control proposed by Max(66 Max L. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 357-388. Stuttering and internal models for sensorimotor control: a theoretical perspective to generate testable hypotheses.) suggests that, for accurate control of all the information involved during speech production (motor, auditory, and somatosensory), the central nervous system maintains internal representations of the motor sequences used. These representations, or internal models, are the basis for speech motor control.

The internal representations are utilized in speech motor control by two processes - the forward process and the inverse process. The forward process predicts likely sensory consequences of a motor command being generated for speech, whereas the inverse process uses these predictions of sensory consequences of central commands and plans what is needed to achieve them. If the likely sensorimotor command differs from the intended command, the internal models are updated for future attempts.

According to this model, stuttering develops in childhood owing to a problem in the acquisition and/or refinement of these internal representations, that is, internal models. Therefore, the incompatibility between the motor commands generated and the inaccurate internal models leads to repetitive attempts to reconfigure the motor planning, generating interruptions in speech performance. The difficulty in learning, consolidating and in the update between the motor command and the consequence of movement would be one the possible causes for involuntary speech disruptions, with no capacity of automatic recovery, characterized by this disorder(66 Max L. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 357-388. Stuttering and internal models for sensorimotor control: a theoretical perspective to generate testable hypotheses.).

Speech fluency improvement can occur from the implementation of changes in the motor patterns of speech production. These changes are achieved through motor learning, whose benefit is associated with the amount of practice. This effect becomes evident during the so-called “adaptation”, i.e., improvement in fluency as a result of repetition of the same sequence of speech. The same sequence of articulatory and phonatory movements enables the prediction of more refined sensory consequences of that movement or sequence of movements and, therefore, a more efficient selection of the necessary motor commands(77 Max L, Baldwin CJ. The role of motor learning in stuttering adaptation: repeated versus novel utterances in a practice-retention paradigm. J Fluency Disord. 2010;35(1):33-43. PMid:20412981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.12.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2009...
).

The melodic function is another mechanism that allows speech fluency improvement based on decreased speech motor control dependence on the internal model. Prosody is a resource of human expression that aims to provide more efficient and appropriate communication from the transmission of paralinguistic information such as tone, intonation, stress, and length(88 Falk S, Maslow E, Thum G, Hoole P. Temporal variability in sung productions of adolescents who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2016;62:101-14. PMid:27323225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.05.012.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016...
,99 Boutsen F. Prosody: the music of language and speech. ASHA Lead. 2003;5:7-9.). When a predetermined melody or speech rhythm is used, clues of length, frequency, and intensity are available and used by the speech motor control system, facilitating fluency.

Little research has been conducted on the difference in motor control between speech tasks so far. It is known that there is a difference in the manifestations of stuttering observed during speech tasks, but there are no definitive studies on the theme. Furthermore, knowledge about the correlation between different speech tasks and their effects on the speech performance of individuals who stutter and fluent speakers is still limited.

The objective of the present study was to compare the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter during the tasks of spontaneous speech, automatic speech, and singing. The hypothesis of this study was that tasks of higher motor and melodic complexities negatively affect the speech fluency of both fluent speakers and individuals who stutter.

METHODS

Participants

The study project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the “Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo” – FMUSP under protocol no. 265/14. All participants signed an Informed Consent Form (ICF) prior to study commencement.

The study sample was composed of 17 adult individuals (14 males; 3 females) with developmental stuttering, aged 19 to 47 years (mean=31.02; SD=8.90), and 17 adult fluent speakers matched for age and gender to the participants with developmental stuttering.

All selected participants were native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese, high-school graduates, without any other oral communication disorders(1010 de Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF, editores. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2004.), hearing loss of any degree, and neurological and/or degenerative diseases.

Inclusion criteria for the Study Group (SG) (adults with developmental stuttering) were as follows: Fluency Profile Assessment Protocol - FPAP scores(1111 Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.) outside the age reference values(1111 Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.) and score ≥25 points in the Stuttering Severity Instrument – Third Edition (SSI-3)(1212 Riley GD. The stuttering severity instrument for adults and children - SSI-3. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1994.), characterizing stuttering of minimal moderate degree. Exclusion criteria for the Control Group (CG) (adult fluent speakers) comprised Fluency Profile Assessment Protocol - FPAP scores(1111 Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.) within the age reference values(1212 Riley GD. The stuttering severity instrument for adults and children - SSI-3. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1994.) and score <10 points in the SSI-3(1212 Riley GD. The stuttering severity instrument for adults and children - SSI-3. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1994.).

For confirmation of the inclusion criteria, the participants were submitted to basic audiological evaluation, anamnesis, and speech fluency assessment (Fluency Profile Assessment Protocol - FPAP(1111 Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.) and SSI-3 tests(1212 Riley GD. The stuttering severity instrument for adults and children - SSI-3. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1994.)).

Procedure

The same methodology was used for the collection and analysis of speech samples in both groups. Speech sample collection was conducted with individuals seated in front of a digital camcorder (Sony DRC-SR62) attached to a tripod (Targus TG5060TR).

Spontaneous speech task

In the monologue task, speech samples were obtained from a stimulus figure according to the methodology proposed in the Fluency Profile Assessment Protocol - FPAP(1111 Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.). Participants were asked to speak freely about the presented figure and could expand their considerations according to their interest.

Automatic speech task

As for the automatic speech, participants were requested to say out loud the days of the week, months, and count from one to 10.

Singing task

In the collection of the singing samples, participants were asked to sing the song “Happy Birthday”.

Analysis of speech samples

Transcription and analysis of the speech samples were conducted by an experienced speech-language therapist with expertise in the field based on the visualization made available in a portable computer (Sony Vaio VPC-AS) using headphones (Maxwell HP200F). The transcription was performed according to the standardized methodology described in the Fluency Profile Assessment Protocol - FPAP(1111 Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.).

All samples were transcribed literally and the episodes of speech disruption were marked. Subsequently, these episodes were classified according to their typology: common disruptions (hesitations, interjections, revisions, unfinished words, repetitions of words) and stuttering-like disruptions (repetitions of syllables and sounds, stretching of sounds, locking, pauses, and intrusions of non-relevant sounds or segments).

Statistical analysis

The study data were collected and submitted to statistical analysis using the SPSS 21 software. Nonparametric tests were used because the distribution of data was not normal for all variables. In addition to descriptive analysis, nonparametric inferential analysis was performed using Friedman's ANOVA and the Dunn test to compare the tasks in each studied variable (intragroup analysis) and the Mann-Whitney test for comparison between groups (intergroup analysis). A level of significance of 5% was adopted for all statistical analyses.

RESULTS

Intragroup comparison - Study Group

Table 1 shows that both the number of common disruptions and the number of stuttering-like disruptions differed significantly between the speech tasks in the Study Group (SG).

Table 1
Comparison of the total number of common and stuttering-like speech disruptions between the different tasks applied to the Study Group

A more comprehensive comparison, presented in Table 2, shows that the monologue task differed significantly from the tasks of automatic speech and singing; however, no statistically significant difference was observed between the tasks of automatic speech and singing.

Table 2
Paired comparison between the tasks applied to the Study Group for the variables total number of common and stuttering-like speech disruptions

Intragroup comparison - Control Group

Table 3 shows that the number of disruptions differed significantly between the speech tasks in the Control Group (CG).

Table 3
Comparison of the total number of common and stuttering-like speech disruptions between the different tasks applied to the Control Group

A comparison based on the Dunn test, presented in Table 4, shows that the number of common disruptions in the monologue task differed significantly from the tasks of automatic speech and singing; however, no statistically significant difference was observed between the tasks of automatic speech and singing. Table 3 shows that there were no stuttering-like disruptions in none of the speech tasks in the CG.

Table 4
Paired comparison of the tasks applied to the Control Group for the variables total number of common and stuttering-like speech disruptions

Intergroup comparison

The number of common and stuttering-like disfluencies was higher for all speech tasks assessed in the SG, as shown in Table 5. Monologue was the speech task that presented the greatest difference between the groups. The tasks of automatic speech and singing presented similar results in both groups.

Table 5
Comparison between the Study (SG) and Control (CG) Groups for the variables total number of common and stuttering-like speech disruptions

DISCUSSION

This study compared the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter in three speech tasks: monologue, automatic speech, and singing. The hypothesis that tasks of higher motor and melodic complexities would negatively affect the speech fluency of both fluent speakers and individuals who stutter was confirmed.

Difference was observed between the tasks of singing and automatic speech compared with the monologue task; however, no difference was found between the first two tasks in both groups. In speech tasks without self-expressive components, i.e., automatic speech and singing, the content is pre-defined and the speech rhythm is melodically marked, leading to greater speech fluency(1313 Bloostein O. A rating scale study of conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent. J Speech Hear Disord. 1950;15(1):29-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.1501.29.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.1501.29...
,1414 Fox PT, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Hirsch TB, Downs JH, Martin C, et al. A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering. Nature. 1996;382(6587):158-61. PMid:8700204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382158a0.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382158a0...
).

In the specific literature, there is consensus that the frequency of stuttering is variable but predictable in different speech tasks(1313 Bloostein O. A rating scale study of conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent. J Speech Hear Disord. 1950;15(1):29-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.1501.29.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.1501.29...
,1414 Fox PT, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Hirsch TB, Downs JH, Martin C, et al. A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering. Nature. 1996;382(6587):158-61. PMid:8700204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382158a0.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382158a0...
). Adaptation is among the aspects of predictability. According to the Internal Model for Sensorimotor Control(55 Perez HR, Stoeckle JH. Stuttering: clinical and research update. Can Fam Physician. 2016;62(6):479-84. PMid:27303004.), repetition of the same speech sequence would update and refine the existing internal model, thus facilitating fluency. Neurophysiological studies on developmental stuttering indicate that the occurrence of a simultaneous fluent model, or of a reduction in the linguistic and motor demand for speech, favor the efficient timing of speech programs. This allows the brain to organize the motor and language functions in individuals who stutter, providing comfortable speaking fluency(1515 Bloodstein O, Bernstein-Ratner N. A handbook on Stuttering. 6. ed. Clifton Park: Cengage Learning; 2008. Capítulo 11, Stuttering as a response: some controversialphenomena.).

The adaptation effect may therefore reflect motor learning associated with the repeated practice of speech motor sequences(1515 Bloodstein O, Bernstein-Ratner N. A handbook on Stuttering. 6. ed. Clifton Park: Cengage Learning; 2008. Capítulo 11, Stuttering as a response: some controversialphenomena.). A study has tested this hypothesis with a paradigm that used two approaches to identify the role of motor learning in the effect of adaptation on stuttering. The study distinguished the practice effects from the situational effects. To this end, the utilized texts contained repeated sentences and new sentences. To differentiate the learning effects from the temporary performance effects, the stuttering frequency was determined for the initial reading after two and 24 hours. Individuals who were able to adapt presented decreased frequency of stuttering-like disruptions in both repeated and new sentences. Nevertheless, the decrease in the rate of stuttering-like disruptions was greater for repeated sentences. The rate of stuttering-like disruptions was again similar for both types of sentences after 2 hours. After 24 hours, no improvement was observed regarding the new sentences, whereas retention of the repeated sentences was detected. The results revealed the presence of the motor learning effect for previously non-experienced sequences of movements(77 Max L, Baldwin CJ. The role of motor learning in stuttering adaptation: repeated versus novel utterances in a practice-retention paradigm. J Fluency Disord. 2010;35(1):33-43. PMid:20412981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.12.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2009...
).

Singing is another aspect of predictability. The deficit in speech performance would be the result of the mismatch between the predicted sensory consequences and the generated sensory consequences, leading the system to perform repeated attempts to finalize the planned movement or to restart the process. The literature suggests that rhythm provides an external clue of the timing of each syllable(66 Max L. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 357-388. Stuttering and internal models for sensorimotor control: a theoretical perspective to generate testable hypotheses.).

Another factor that contributes for singing to favor speech fluency is the fact that during singing there is a decrease in the articulatory velocity, with increased phonation interval(1616 Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Bothe AK, Wang Y, Kilgo M. Efficacy of the modifying phonation intervals (MPI) stuttering treatment program with adults who stutter. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2015;24(2):256-71. PMid:25633470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0076.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-...
). Decreasing the phonation interval has been one of the great indicators for improving speech fluency in individuals who stutter(1717 Davidow JH. Systematic studies of modified vocalization: the effect of speech rate on speech production measures during metronome-paced speech in persons who stutter. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):100-12. PMid:24372888. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12050.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.1205...
).

Regarding the intergroup comparison, the only statistically significant difference found was related to the monologue task. This outcome was expected considering that this is the task that distinguishes fluent speakers from individuals who stutter. This difference occurs because individuals who stutter present deficits in processing and/or in sensorimotor and learning integration, which leads to difficulties in the temporal control of movement(66 Max L. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 357-388. Stuttering and internal models for sensorimotor control: a theoretical perspective to generate testable hypotheses.,1818 Namasivayam AK, Van Lieshout P. Speech motor skill and stuttering. J Mot Behav. 2011;43(6):477-89. PMid:22106825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2011.628347.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2011....
,1919 Daliri A, Prokopenko RA, Max L. Afferent and efferent aspects of mandibular sensorimotor control in adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56(6):1774-88. PMid:23816664. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0134).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013...
). The tasks of automatic speech and singing approximated the two groups and presented similar results (there was no statistically significant difference between the groups), which is also in compliance with the literature(22 Yairi E, Ambrose N. Epidemiology of Stuttering: 21st Century Advances. J Fluency Disord. 2013;38(2):66-87. PMid:23773662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.11.002.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012...
,66 Max L. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 357-388. Stuttering and internal models for sensorimotor control: a theoretical perspective to generate testable hypotheses.).

CONCLUSION

The outcomes of this study indicate that tasks of higher motor and melodic complexities, such as the monologue task, negatively affect the speech fluency of both fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. Further studies on the speech variability of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter should be conducted in order to find clues relevant to a better understanding of stuttering and its possible causes.

  • Study carried out at Laboratório de Investigação Fonoaudiológica em Fluência, Funções da Face e Disfagia, Departamento de Fisioterapia, Fonoaudiologia e Terapia Ocupacional, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo – USP - São Paulo (SP), Brazil.
  • Financial support: Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP), process number 2014/09404-0.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Bloodstein O. Incipient and developed stuttering as two distinct disorders: resolving a dilemma. J Fluency Disord. 2001;26(1):67-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0094-730X(00)00077-2
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0094-730X(00)00077-2
  • 2
    Yairi E, Ambrose N. Epidemiology of Stuttering: 21st Century Advances. J Fluency Disord. 2013;38(2):66-87. PMid:23773662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.11.002
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.11.002
  • 3
    Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. PMid:15159193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
  • 4
    Packman A, Code C, Onslow M. On the cause of stuttering: integrating theory with brain and behavioral research. J Neurolinguist. 2007;20(5):353-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.11.001
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.11.001
  • 5
    Perez HR, Stoeckle JH. Stuttering: clinical and research update. Can Fam Physician. 2016;62(6):479-84. PMid:27303004.
  • 6
    Max L. Speech motor control in normal and disordered speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 357-388. Stuttering and internal models for sensorimotor control: a theoretical perspective to generate testable hypotheses.
  • 7
    Max L, Baldwin CJ. The role of motor learning in stuttering adaptation: repeated versus novel utterances in a practice-retention paradigm. J Fluency Disord. 2010;35(1):33-43. PMid:20412981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.12.003
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.12.003
  • 8
    Falk S, Maslow E, Thum G, Hoole P. Temporal variability in sung productions of adolescents who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2016;62:101-14. PMid:27323225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.05.012
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.05.012
  • 9
    Boutsen F. Prosody: the music of language and speech. ASHA Lead. 2003;5:7-9.
  • 10
    de Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF, editores. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2004.
  • 11
    Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. CD-ROM.
  • 12
    Riley GD. The stuttering severity instrument for adults and children - SSI-3. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1994.
  • 13
    Bloostein O. A rating scale study of conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent. J Speech Hear Disord. 1950;15(1):29-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.1501.29
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.1501.29
  • 14
    Fox PT, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Hirsch TB, Downs JH, Martin C, et al. A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering. Nature. 1996;382(6587):158-61. PMid:8700204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382158a0
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382158a0
  • 15
    Bloodstein O, Bernstein-Ratner N. A handbook on Stuttering. 6. ed. Clifton Park: Cengage Learning; 2008. Capítulo 11, Stuttering as a response: some controversialphenomena.
  • 16
    Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Bothe AK, Wang Y, Kilgo M. Efficacy of the modifying phonation intervals (MPI) stuttering treatment program with adults who stutter. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2015;24(2):256-71. PMid:25633470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0076
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0076
  • 17
    Davidow JH. Systematic studies of modified vocalization: the effect of speech rate on speech production measures during metronome-paced speech in persons who stutter. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):100-12. PMid:24372888. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12050
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12050
  • 18
    Namasivayam AK, Van Lieshout P. Speech motor skill and stuttering. J Mot Behav. 2011;43(6):477-89. PMid:22106825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2011.628347
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2011.628347
  • 19
    Daliri A, Prokopenko RA, Max L. Afferent and efferent aspects of mandibular sensorimotor control in adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56(6):1774-88. PMid:23816664. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0134)
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0134)

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    2017

History

  • Received
    13 July 2016
  • Accepted
    29 Oct 2016
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br