Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Indicators of speech fluency in stuttering and in phonological disorder

ABSTRACT

Purpose

To identify the indicators of speech fluency that differs subjects with stuttering, with phonological disorder, and with the two disorders in comorbidity.

Methods

Thirty subjects aged 4-11 years old were divided into 3 groups, each one with 10 subjects: groups with developmental stuttering (SG), phonological disorder (PDG), and with two diagnoses in comorbidity (SPDG) participated in this study. The procedures were speech fluency and phonology evaluation. The data were submitted to statistical analysis.

Results

Subjects from SG and SPDG showed a greater occurrence of stuttering-like disfluencies and total of disfluencies in relation to the subjects with PDG. Regarding to the other disfluencies, the three groups were similar. Subjects with PDG showed fewer monosyllabic word repetitions, part of word repetition and prolongations in relation to subjects from SG and SPDG. Blocks occurred more frequently in the two groups with stuttering (SG and SPDG) than in the group with PDG. Interjection occurred more frequently in subjects from SG than in PDG.

Conclusion

The PDG was the most differentiated in quantitative and qualitative terms in the three groups analyzed. The similarities and differences between the groups will assist the differential diagnosis and, consequently, will enable improved therapy. The presence of blocks represents an important marker for the diagnosis of stuttering

Keywords:
Stuttering; Childhood-onset Fluency Disorder; Phonological Disorder; Speech; Comorbidity

RESUMO

Objetivo

Identificar os indicadores de fluência da fala que diferenciam os sujeitos com gagueira, com transtorno fonológico e com os dois distúrbios em comorbidade.

Método

Participaram deste estudo 30 sujeitos de 4 a 11 anos, separados em 3 grupos, cada um com 10 sujeitos: grupo com gagueira do desenvolvimento (GG), transtorno fonológico (GTF) e os dois diagnósticos em comorbidade (GGTF). Os procedimentos foram: avaliação da fluência da fala e da fonologia. Os dados foram submetidos à análise estatística.

Resultados

Os sujeitos do GG e GGTF apresentaram maior ocorrência das disfluências típicas da gagueira e do total das disfluências em relação aos do GTF. Em relação às outras disfluências, os três grupos foram semelhantes. O GTF manifestou menor quantidade de repetições de palavra monossilábica, de parte de palavra e prolongamentos em relação aos sujeitos dos GG e GGTF. Os bloqueios ocorreram mais frequentemente nos dois grupos com gagueira (GG e GGTF) em relação ao GTF. A interjeição ocorreu com maior frequência no GG quando comparado com o GTF.

Conclusão

Dos três grupos analisados, o GTF foi o que mais se diferenciou em termos quantitativo e qualitativo. As semelhanças e diferenças entre os grupos auxiliarão o diagnóstico diferencial e, consequentemente, possibilitarão melhor terapia. A presença de bloqueio representa um importante marcador para o diagnóstico de gagueira.

Descritores:
Gagueira; Transtorno da Fluência com Início na Infância; Transtorno Fonológico; Fala; Comorbidade

INTRODUCTION

Stuttering is a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental, complex and multifactorial fluency disorder, characterized by frequent disruptions in the flow of speech(11 Chang S, Garnetti EO, Etchell A, Chow HO. Functional and neuroanatomical bases of developmental stuttering: current insights. Neuroscientist. 2019;25(6):566-82. PMid:30264661.). Linguistic, cognitive and motor skills may be related to the disorder(22 Smith A, Weber C. How stuttering develops: the multifactorial dynamic pathways theory. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60(9):2483-505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0343. PMid:28837728.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-1...
), and especially the phonological variable has received considerable attention in the literature(33 Sasisekaran J. Exploring the link between stuttering and phonology: a review and implications for treatment. Semin Speech Lang. 2014;35(2):95-113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1371754. PMid:24782273.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1371754...
).

The Covert Repair Hypothesis theory(44 Postma A, Kolk H. The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(3):472-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472. PMid:8331905.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472...
) proposes that delayed phonological encoding in persons who stutter leads to an increased internal correction of coding errors, causing stuttering events. It is also believed that higher phonological demand is impairs the stability of the vulnerable motor system in persons who stutter(22 Smith A, Weber C. How stuttering develops: the multifactorial dynamic pathways theory. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60(9):2483-505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0343. PMid:28837728.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-1...
). Therefore, some stuttering theories suggest that difficulty in phonological encoding may delay or disrupt planning and subsequent articulatory execution, causing disfluencies(55 Tsai PT. Phonological neighborhood effect in spontaneous speech in adults who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2018;58:86-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2018.08.005. PMid:30180994.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2018...
).

Briefly, phonological disorder in children who stutter co-occurs at a substantial rate: it is estimated that from 16 to 30% of children who stutter also have phonological disorder, while the prevalence in the general population is from 6 to 8%(66 Blood GW, Ridenour VJ Jr, Qualls CD, Hammer CS. Co-occurring disorders in children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2003;36(6):427-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00023-6. PMid:12967738.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)...
,77 Louko LJ, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Phonological characteristics of young stutterers and their normally fluent peers: preliminary observations. J Fluency Disord. 1990;15(4):191-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)90001-9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)9...
). Despite the frequent reference in literature to the coexistence of disfluencies in children with phonological disorder, there are few studies that describe in detail the manifestations of disfluencies that may differentiate these two conditions(77 Louko LJ, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Phonological characteristics of young stutterers and their normally fluent peers: preliminary observations. J Fluency Disord. 1990;15(4):191-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)90001-9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)9...
).

In order to highlight the co-occurrence of phonological disorder and stuttering, a study showed that children with phonological disorder often have disfluencies or hesitantion marks, for example hesitations, reformulations, repetitions, prologations, silent and filled pauses, beyond false beginnings(88 Berti LC, Marino VCC. Marcas linguísticas constitutivas do processo de aquisição do contraste fônico. Rev GEL. 2008;5(2):103-21.). However, although the authors of this research describe the presence of disfluencies in children with phonological disorders, the interpretation given to disfluencies is that they would be constitutive in the process of phonic acquisition(88 Berti LC, Marino VCC. Marcas linguísticas constitutivas do processo de aquisição do contraste fônico. Rev GEL. 2008;5(2):103-21.).

Although studies indicate the presence of disfluencies in children with phonological disorders(77 Louko LJ, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Phonological characteristics of young stutterers and their normally fluent peers: preliminary observations. J Fluency Disord. 1990;15(4):191-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)90001-9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)9...
,88 Berti LC, Marino VCC. Marcas linguísticas constitutivas do processo de aquisição do contraste fônico. Rev GEL. 2008;5(2):103-21.), the number of investigations that deepen the analysis of disfluencies is restricted, and consequently little is known about similarities and differences between the disruptions that occur in these two disorders.

In particular, it is assumed that: (H1) subjects with stuttering and subjects with both disorders in comorbidity would have a higher frequency of occurrence as regarding the stuttering-like disfluencies as in relation to other disfluencies compared to those with phonological disorder; (H2) Regarding the typology of disfluencies, there would be differences between subjects with phonological disorder compared to subjects with stuttering and those with stuttering and phonological disorder in comorbidity, that is, subjects with phonological disorder would predominantly have other disfluencies, while subjects with stuttering and with stuttering and phonological disorder would predominantly have the stuttering-like disfluencies.

It is believed that the details of manifestations regarding disfluencies showed by individuals with isolated disorders, on the one hand, and others with disorders in comorbidity, on the other hand, may assist in the differential diagnosis between the conditions and, consequently, favor the best therapeutic conduct.

Assuming that subjects who stutter and subjects with phonological disorder could show differences both regarding the frequency of occurrence and the typology of disfluencies, this study aimed to identify indicators of speech fluency among subjects who stutter, with phonological disorder and those with the two disorders in comorbidity.

METHODS

Ethical considerations

This is a cross-sectional and prospective observational research with comparison between groups. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee (nº 2.070.227). It is noteworthy that all the requirements governing the Resolution of the National Health Council - No. 466/2012 were respected, and the subjects had their participation authorized by signing the Informed Consent Term.

Participants

Data collection to compose the sample occurred during the first semester of 2017. The sample consisted of 30 subjects, both genders, aged 4 to 11 years and 11 months, coming from the Acoustic Analysis Laboratory – (Laboratório de Análise Acústica) LAAc and Fluency Studies Laboratory – (Laboratório de Estudos da Fluência) LAEF, Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, Faculty of Philosophy and Science, São Paulo State University “Júlio de Mesquita Filho” - UNESP - Marília. Participants were divided into three groups composed of ten subjects each: group with developmental stuttering (SG); group with the diagnosis of phonological disorder (PDG); and group with the two diagnoses in comorbidity (SPDG).

Inclusion criteria for this study were monolingual native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese, without previous speech therapy, aged 4 to 11 years and 11 months. The mean age of SG was 8.40; of the PDG was 6.70; and at SPDG was 6.10 years.

For SG, those who met the diagnostic criterion for persistent developmental stuttering with at least 3% of stuttering-like disfluencies(99 Gregg BA, Yairi E. Disfluency patterns and phonological skills near stuttering onset. J Commun Disord. 2012;45(6):426-38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.001. PMid:22939524.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012...
,1010 Tumanova V, Conture EG, Lambert EW, Walden TA. Speech disfluencies of preschool-age children who do and do not stutter. J Commun Disord. 2014;49:25-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.01.003. PMid:24503151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014...
) and 12 months of duration of the disfluencies were included.

Specifically, for the PDG, after phonological evaluation, those diagnosed with phonological disorder and who showed spontaneous speech with less than 3% of stuttering-like disfluencies were included; therefore, they did not meet the diagnostic criteria for stuttering.

The diagnosis of phonological disorder was based on the analysis of phonological processes, according to the age at which they should be suppressed, that is, the presence of persistent phonological processes, no longer expected for the child's age group, associated with complementary diagnostic tests, such as: speech perception evaluation, oral diadochokinesia test, multisyllabic word repetition, lexical accent test, among others.

The third and last group included subjects diagnosed with persistent developmental stuttering in comorbidity with phonological disorder.

Those who had neurological, auditory, behavioral, learning disorders, mental disability, genetic syndromes, psychiatric conditions or other relevant alterations that could influence the manifestations of disfluencies or those who, after the collection and transcription of the speech sample did not have 200 fluent syllables, were excluded from this research. Data regarding various possibilities of alterations that could influence the final diagnosis were obtained by analyzing the medical records of each participant, at the time of admission to the service provided by the School Clinic, were screened and respectively diagnosed.

Procedures

The research procedures were distributed in two stages: speech fluency evaluation and phonology evaluation.

For the speech fluency evaluation, conducted with all sample subjects, an analysis of the spontaneous speech sample was choosen, which constitutes of a greater complexity task for both motor and linguistic(1111 Costa JB, Ritto AP, Juste FS, Andrade CRF. Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. CoDAS. 2017;29(2):e20160136. PMid:28327784.), as well as favors the occurrence of disfluencies in speech when compared to singing, reading, directed speech, among others(1212 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001. PMid:15159193.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004...
).

For data collection, digital audiovisual recordings were made of each subject, in interaction with the evaluator. Speech samples were analyzed and transcribed in a total of 200 fluent syllables. Disfluencies were characterized into Stuttering-like Disfluencies (monosyllabic word repetition, syllable repetition, sound repetition, block, prolongation, pause, intrusion) and Other Disfluencies (interjection, hesitation, revision, unfinished words, phrase repetition, non-monosyllabic word repetition), as proposed in the literature(1313 Ambrose N, Yairi E. Normative disfluency data for early childhood stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(4):895-909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4204.895. PMid:10450909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4204.895...
).

To determine the frequency of disruptions, the following measures were used: Stuttering-Like Disfluencies (SLD), Other Disfluencies (OD), and Total of Disfluency (TD). To calculate the percentage of SLD, the total number of SLD events was multiplied by 100 and divided by 200, that is, the total of fluent syllables. The same calculations were performed with the total of OD and total of disfluencies (TD, that is, the sum of the SLD and the OD).

For phonological evaluation of PDG isolated or in comorbidity with stuttering, the Speech Evaluation Instrument for Acoustical Analysis (Instrumento de Avaliação de Fala para Análise Acústica - IAFAC) was applied(1414 Berti LC, Pagliuso A, Lacava F. Speech evaluation instrument for acoustical analysis based on linguistic criteria. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(3):305-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342009000300005.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342009...
). This instrument contains 96 figures, which encompasses the production of words containing all phonemes of the Brazilian Portuguese phonological system, in the context of the vowels /i, a, u/, regarding both the position of simple attack, complex attack, and the position of coda syllable. The data collected through the IAFAC instrument were recorded and later phonetically transcribed.

Statistical analysis

A descriptive and inferential statistical analysis of the data was conducted using STATISTICA 7.0 software. In the comparison between groups, the one-way ANOVA parametric test was used, adopting as independent variable the three groups of subjects (SG, PDG and SPDG), and as dependent variables, the total of Stuttering-Like Disfluencies (SLD) and the total of other types of disfluencies (OD). In cases where there was a significant effect for the group, the Bonferroni post-hoc test was used to verify which groups differed.

When comparing the groups, considering each variable analyzed separately (stuttering-like disfluencies: monosyllabic word repetition, part of the word repetition, sound repetition, prolongations, blocks, pauses and intrusions; and other disfluencies: hesitation, interjection, revision, segment repetition, phrase repetition, non-monosyllabic word repetition and unfinished word), the Kruskal-Wallis nonparametric test was used for multiple comparisons.

RESULTS

When comparing the frequency of Stuttering-Like Disfluencies (SLD) and Other Disfluencies (OD) among the three groups studied, Anova One-Way showed a statistical difference between the groups for SLD (F (2.27) = 14.83, p<0.00) and total of disfluencies (TD) (F (2.27) =9.74, p<0.00), not for total of OD (F (2.27) =1.98, p=0.15) (Figure 1). Then, a post-hoc analysis was performed using the Bonferroni test to verify which groups differed. It was found that the PDG subjects had a lower frequency of occurrence of SLD and TD compared to SG and SPDG groups (p <0.00), but there was no difference between SG and SPDG groups (p=0.17 for SLD and p=0.78 for TD).

Figure 1
Graph of comparison by pairs for the frequency of Stuttering-Like Disfluencies, Other Disfluencies and Total of Disfluencies. Note: Keys with the asterisks represent the groups that differed from each other. Mean +/- standard error of disfluency event averages of participating groups. *Statistical difference (p <0.05) - Bonferroni test. Source: Created by the author

Regarding the typology of disfluencies, it was observed (Table 1), difference of PDG and SPDG in the following variables: “monosyllabic word repetition” (H (2.30) =11.73, p=<0.00); “part of the word repetition” (H (2.30) =8.70, p=<0.01); and “prolongation” (H (2.30) =8.98, p=<0.01). Regarding SLD, for the “block” variable, the PDG is different from both SG and SPDG, (H (2.30) =12.63, p=<0.00), showing a lower frequency of occurrence.

Table 1
Comparison by typology of Stuttering-Like Disfluencies as a function of groups

The comparison of the frequency of occurrence of the typologies for other disfluencies as a function of groups is shown in Table 2. Regarding the typologies analyzed in the OD (hesitation, interjection, revision, segment repetition, phrase repetition, non-monosyllabic word repetition and unfinished word), only the variable “interjection” differentiated the PDG from the SG (H (2.30)=12.22, p=0.02).

Table 2
Comparison of the frequency of typologies of other disfluencies as a function of groups.

DISCUSSION

This study analyzed and compared the frequency and typology of disfluencies in subjects with stuttering, phonological disorder and with both disorders in comorbidity.

The first hypothesis raised was partially confirmed, regarding that the SG and SPDG showed a higher occurrence of stuttering-like disfluencies when compared to the PDG subjects, however the groups were similar regarding the other disfluencies.

The lower frequency of occurrence of SLD in PDG seems to be related mainly to the fluency profile of fluent subjects, since the stuttering-like disfluencies are uncommon in fluent subjects(1515 Martins VO, Andrade CRF. Speech fluency developmental profile in brazilian Portuguese speakers. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(1):7-12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008...
) and do not characterize the manifestations showed by subjects with the phonological disorder.

Subjects from SG and SPDG showed similarities as to the frequency of occurrence of SLD and OD. This finding confirms that SLD characterize the manifestations of stuttering(1616 Hudock D, Kalinowski J. Stuttering inhibition via altered auditory feedback during scripted telephone conversations. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):139-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12053. PMid:24372890.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.1205...

17 Kronfeld-Duenias V, Ezrati-Vinacour R, Civier O, Ben-Shachar M. The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;14:912-8.

18 Vanhoutte S, Santens P, Cosyns M, van Mierlo P, Batens K, Corthals P, et al. Increased motor preparation activity during fluent single word production in DS: a correlate for stuttering frequency and severity. J Fluency Disord. 2015;75:1-10. PMid:26004061.

19 Tumanova V, Zebrowski PM, Goodman SS, Arenas RM. Motor practice effects and sensorimotor integration in adults who stutter: evidence from visuomotor tracking performance. J Fluency Disord. 2015;45:52-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.04.001. PMid:25990027.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015...
-2020 Ambrose NG, Yairi E, Loucks TM, Seery CH, Throneburg R. Relation of motor, linguistic and temperamento factors in epidemiologic subtypes of persistente and recovered stuttering: initial findings. J Fluency Disord. 2015;45:12-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.05.004. PMid:26117417.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015...
), regardless it is or not associated with phonological disorder. Regarding this result, although SPDG is composed of subjects with disorders in comorbidity, as the quantitative profile of SLD and OD, the group was similar to that found in SG.

This finding corroborates literature descriptions that the excess of stuttering-like disfluencies is the main characteristic of the disorder(1616 Hudock D, Kalinowski J. Stuttering inhibition via altered auditory feedback during scripted telephone conversations. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):139-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12053. PMid:24372890.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.1205...

17 Kronfeld-Duenias V, Ezrati-Vinacour R, Civier O, Ben-Shachar M. The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;14:912-8.

18 Vanhoutte S, Santens P, Cosyns M, van Mierlo P, Batens K, Corthals P, et al. Increased motor preparation activity during fluent single word production in DS: a correlate for stuttering frequency and severity. J Fluency Disord. 2015;75:1-10. PMid:26004061.

19 Tumanova V, Zebrowski PM, Goodman SS, Arenas RM. Motor practice effects and sensorimotor integration in adults who stutter: evidence from visuomotor tracking performance. J Fluency Disord. 2015;45:52-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.04.001. PMid:25990027.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015...
-2020 Ambrose NG, Yairi E, Loucks TM, Seery CH, Throneburg R. Relation of motor, linguistic and temperamento factors in epidemiologic subtypes of persistente and recovered stuttering: initial findings. J Fluency Disord. 2015;45:12-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.05.004. PMid:26117417.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015...
), and these disruptions constitute one of the most important parameters for diagnosis of stuttering.

Given that speech is the main form of human communication and that its efficiency in transferring information depends on fluency(2121 Esmaili I, Dabanloo NJ, Vali M. Automatic classification of speech dysfluencies in continuous speech based on similarity measures and morphological image processing tools. Biomed Signal Process Control. 2016;23:104-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.08.006.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.08...
), subjects with stuttering or with stuttering and phonological disorder in comorbidity show significant impairments in communication. It should be emphasized that stuttering is not a simple speech difficulty, but a serious communication problem(2222 Crinion JT. Facilitating fluency in adults who stutter. Brain. 2018;141(4):944-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy075. PMid:29596670.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy075...
).

Therefore, this finding, added to the fact that 16 to 30% of children who stutter also have phonological disorders(66 Blood GW, Ridenour VJ Jr, Qualls CD, Hammer CS. Co-occurring disorders in children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2003;36(6):427-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00023-6. PMid:12967738.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)...
,77 Louko LJ, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Phonological characteristics of young stutterers and their normally fluent peers: preliminary observations. J Fluency Disord. 1990;15(4):191-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)90001-9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)9...
), should be considered by the speech therapist in the diagnostic process and, consequently, for the planning of a personalized therapy that meets the real needs of each patient. A subject with impaired intelligibility, which may occur due to phonological disorder(2323 Wertzner HF, Amaro L, Galea DE. Phonological performance measured by speech severity indices compared with correlated factors. Sao Paulo Med J. 2007;125(6):309-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802007000600002. PMid:18317599.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802007...
), added to the involuntary stuttering-like disfluencies that occur in the flow of speech, will show greater communication impairment and be more susceptible to bullying. As described in the literature(33 Sasisekaran J. Exploring the link between stuttering and phonology: a review and implications for treatment. Semin Speech Lang. 2014;35(2):95-113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1371754. PMid:24782273.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1371754...
), this may be an important subgroup of persons who stutter that deserve better attention from researchers.

The analysis and quantitative comparison of the other disfluencies showed that the three groups were similar. It is worth emphasizing that, although the subjects in the three groups have some type of communication disorder, there was no increase in the amount of other disfluencies in relation to the subjects with typical development, based on the reference values indicated in a previous study(1515 Martins VO, Andrade CRF. Speech fluency developmental profile in brazilian Portuguese speakers. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(1):7-12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008...
). These findings are consistent with the results of a study(2424 Costa VP, Albiero JK, Mota HB. Aspects of speech fluency in children with and without evolutive phonological disorder. Rev CEFAC. 2015;17(Supl 1):9-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462011005000063.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462011...
) in which the authors found similarity in the amount of OD between a group with phonological disorder and a control group.

Regarding the language processing, other disfluencies reflect the uncertainties and linguistic inaccuracies, aiming to broaden the comprehension of the message(2525 Seno MP, Giacheti CM, Moretti-Ferreira D. Narrative language and fluency in Down syndrome: a review. Rev CEFAC. 2014;16(4):1311-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201424512.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620142...
), justifying the results of this research. These disfluencies are constitutive of the speech dynamics of any person, since speech planning and execution are complex and involve the connection of many brain areas. They may also indicate a way to alleviate speech difficulties at the moment when subjects with phonological disorders try to reach the target pronunciation(88 Berti LC, Marino VCC. Marcas linguísticas constitutivas do processo de aquisição do contraste fônico. Rev GEL. 2008;5(2):103-21.,2424 Costa VP, Albiero JK, Mota HB. Aspects of speech fluency in children with and without evolutive phonological disorder. Rev CEFAC. 2015;17(Supl 1):9-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462011005000063.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462011...
) or subjects who stutter try to avoid SLD.

The second hypothesis was also partially confirmed, since only certain typologies of the SLD and OD showed significant differences between the groups. In terms of the analyzed typologies of Other Disfluencies, only “interjection” occurred more frequently in SG than in PDG.

Regarding the four typologies of SLD (monosyllabic word repetition, part of the word repetition, prolongation and block), they were less frequent in PDG when compared to SPDG. It seems coherent to explain that the difference found between the groups may be a result of motor complexity during spontaneous speech emission of the SG. In this population, disruptions can be explained by the difference between the generated motor command and the desired motor command during speech, since the greater the motor and melodic complexity during a task, the greater the impairment in speech fluency(1111 Costa JB, Ritto AP, Juste FS, Andrade CRF. Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. CoDAS. 2017;29(2):e20160136. PMid:28327784.).

Regarding the non-differentiation between SG and SPDG for the variables “ monosyllabic word repetition”, “ part of the word repetition” and “prolongation”, it is assumed the presence of dysfunction in the basal ganglia motor circuits, characteristic of the diagnosis of stuttering. Although SPDG concomitantly has the diagnosis of phonological disorder in subjects with stuttering, there is a disorder in the medial system(1212 Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001. PMid:15159193.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004...
), justifying the occurrence of disfluencies and its non-differentiation between the aforesaid groups, since the task chosen for this research was spontaneous speech, in which the dominance of the medial system occurs mainly when speech conveys thoughts or emotions (present in the speech sample, task of this research).

The non-differentiation of these typologies between SG and PDG may have occurred due to a tendency of smaller occurrence of these disfluencies in SG in relation to SPDG. Although the difference is not statistically significant, it is possible to notice that the mean SLD and total of disfluencies (TD) were higher in SPDG when compared to SG.

With regard specifically to the variable “block”, the subjects of the PDG had a lower frequency of occurrence of this typology, both SG and SPDG. Block is a very characteristic typology of the people who stutter’s speech, is often present in the description of the disorder(2626 Garnett EO, Chow HM, Nieto-Castañón A, Tourville JA, Guenther FH, Chang SE. Anomalous morphology in left hemisphere motor and premotor cortex of children who stutter. Brain. 2018;141(9):2670-84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy199. PMid:30084910.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy199...
,2727 Connally EL, Ward D, Pliatsikas C, Finnegan S, Jenkinson M, Boyles R, et al. Separation of trait and state in stuttering. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(8):3109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24063. PMid:29624772.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24063...
) and its presence is very rare in the speech of people who do not manifest stuttering.

Taking into consideration the experiences, feelings and negative attitudes that subjects with communication disorders may show, more specifically subjects who stutter, it is interpreted that one of the possibilities of higher occurrence of “blocks” in SG and SPDG may result from this freezing response transferred to the speech of these subjects who experience negative emotions perceived as threatening and manifesting as a “freezing response” characterized by an inhibition of movement(2828 Erdemir A, Walden TA, Jefferson CM, Choi D, Jones RM. The effect of emotion on articulation rate in persistence and recovery of childhood stuttering. J Fluency Disord. 2018;56:1-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.11.003. PMid:29443691.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2017...
).

With regard to the results obtained, it seems reasonable to suspect that both speech disorders - phonological disorder and developmental stuttering - may share underlying aspects, as proposed in a previous study(2929 Wolk L, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Coexistence of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(5):906-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.906. PMid:8246479.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.906...
).

The first shared aspect refers to the fact that both disorders constitute “communication disorders”. Subjects who have some “communication disorder” may acquire a sense of “failure” as a speaker and consequently, may suffer in their attempts at speech production. The second shared aspect concerns a common predisposition to the phonological disorder and developmental stuttering, which could cause similar manifestations in these conditions. Lastly, it is suspected that a third shared aspect by both disorders would be the presence of a central neurological processing deficit or even a neuromotor disorder or delay, which would lead to interruptions in temporal programming(3030 Caruso AJ, Abbs JH, Gracco VL. Kinematic analysis of multiple movement coordination during speech in stutterers. Brain. 1988;111(2):439-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/111.2.439. PMid:3378144.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/111.2.43...
), providing similar manifestations in the subjects of both groups.

In scientific terms, new study designs may be proposed that contemplate both the analysis of stuttering severity and the analysis of phonological disorder severity. Regarding the clinical implications, it is believed that the speech therapist necessarily needs to analyze the typology of disfluencies to distinguish subjects diagnosed with stuttering, phonological disorder and the two disorders in comorbidity. Moreover, the presence of block represents an important marker for the diagnosis of stuttering.

CONCLUSION

Comparison of the frequency of disfluencies showed that the PDG showed less amount of stuttering-like disfluencies and total disfluencies in relation to SG and SPDG. monosyllabic word repetition, part of the word repetition, and prolongations occurred more frequently in SPDG compared to PDG. Block was less frequent in PDG compared to SG and SPDG.

The three groups were similar in other disfluencies. Interjections were more frequent in SG when compared to PDG.

Since the phonological disorder shows all types of stuttering-like disfluencies, the typology of disruptions seems poorly appropriate as an indicator for the differential diagnosis among the studied conditions, being the frequency with these typologies occur is the best indicator for the differential diagnosis.

  • Study conducted at Laboratório de Estudos da Fluência – LAEF e no Laboratório de Análise Acústica – LAAc, Departamento de Fonoaudiologia, Faculdade de Filosofia e Ciências, Universidade Estadual Paulista “Júlio de Mesquita Filho” – UNESP - Marília (SP), Brasil.
  • Financial support: nothing to declare.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Chang S, Garnetti EO, Etchell A, Chow HO. Functional and neuroanatomical bases of developmental stuttering: current insights. Neuroscientist. 2019;25(6):566-82. PMid:30264661.
  • 2
    Smith A, Weber C. How stuttering develops: the multifactorial dynamic pathways theory. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017;60(9):2483-505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0343 PMid:28837728.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0343
  • 3
    Sasisekaran J. Exploring the link between stuttering and phonology: a review and implications for treatment. Semin Speech Lang. 2014;35(2):95-113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1371754 PMid:24782273.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0034-1371754
  • 4
    Postma A, Kolk H. The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(3):472-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472 PMid:8331905.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472
  • 5
    Tsai PT. Phonological neighborhood effect in spontaneous speech in adults who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2018;58:86-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2018.08.005 PMid:30180994.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2018.08.005
  • 6
    Blood GW, Ridenour VJ Jr, Qualls CD, Hammer CS. Co-occurring disorders in children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2003;36(6):427-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00023-6 PMid:12967738.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00023-6
  • 7
    Louko LJ, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Phonological characteristics of young stutterers and their normally fluent peers: preliminary observations. J Fluency Disord. 1990;15(4):191-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)90001-9
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(90)90001-9
  • 8
    Berti LC, Marino VCC. Marcas linguísticas constitutivas do processo de aquisição do contraste fônico. Rev GEL. 2008;5(2):103-21.
  • 9
    Gregg BA, Yairi E. Disfluency patterns and phonological skills near stuttering onset. J Commun Disord. 2012;45(6):426-38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.001 PMid:22939524.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.001
  • 10
    Tumanova V, Conture EG, Lambert EW, Walden TA. Speech disfluencies of preschool-age children who do and do not stutter. J Commun Disord. 2014;49:25-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.01.003 PMid:24503151.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.01.003
  • 11
    Costa JB, Ritto AP, Juste FS, Andrade CRF. Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. CoDAS. 2017;29(2):e20160136. PMid:28327784.
  • 12
    Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37(4):325-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001 PMid:15159193.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001
  • 13
    Ambrose N, Yairi E. Normative disfluency data for early childhood stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(4):895-909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4204.895 PMid:10450909.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4204.895
  • 14
    Berti LC, Pagliuso A, Lacava F. Speech evaluation instrument for acoustical analysis based on linguistic criteria. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(3):305-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342009000300005
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342009000300005
  • 15
    Martins VO, Andrade CRF. Speech fluency developmental profile in brazilian Portuguese speakers. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(1):7-12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002
  • 16
    Hudock D, Kalinowski J. Stuttering inhibition via altered auditory feedback during scripted telephone conversations. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):139-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12053 PMid:24372890.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12053
  • 17
    Kronfeld-Duenias V, Ezrati-Vinacour R, Civier O, Ben-Shachar M. The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;14:912-8.
  • 18
    Vanhoutte S, Santens P, Cosyns M, van Mierlo P, Batens K, Corthals P, et al. Increased motor preparation activity during fluent single word production in DS: a correlate for stuttering frequency and severity. J Fluency Disord. 2015;75:1-10. PMid:26004061.
  • 19
    Tumanova V, Zebrowski PM, Goodman SS, Arenas RM. Motor practice effects and sensorimotor integration in adults who stutter: evidence from visuomotor tracking performance. J Fluency Disord. 2015;45:52-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.04.001 PMid:25990027.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.04.001
  • 20
    Ambrose NG, Yairi E, Loucks TM, Seery CH, Throneburg R. Relation of motor, linguistic and temperamento factors in epidemiologic subtypes of persistente and recovered stuttering: initial findings. J Fluency Disord. 2015;45:12-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.05.004 PMid:26117417.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2015.05.004
  • 21
    Esmaili I, Dabanloo NJ, Vali M. Automatic classification of speech dysfluencies in continuous speech based on similarity measures and morphological image processing tools. Biomed Signal Process Control. 2016;23:104-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.08.006
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.08.006
  • 22
    Crinion JT. Facilitating fluency in adults who stutter. Brain. 2018;141(4):944-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy075 PMid:29596670.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy075
  • 23
    Wertzner HF, Amaro L, Galea DE. Phonological performance measured by speech severity indices compared with correlated factors. Sao Paulo Med J. 2007;125(6):309-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802007000600002 PMid:18317599.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802007000600002
  • 24
    Costa VP, Albiero JK, Mota HB. Aspects of speech fluency in children with and without evolutive phonological disorder. Rev CEFAC. 2015;17(Supl 1):9-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462011005000063
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462011005000063
  • 25
    Seno MP, Giacheti CM, Moretti-Ferreira D. Narrative language and fluency in Down syndrome: a review. Rev CEFAC. 2014;16(4):1311-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201424512
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201424512
  • 26
    Garnett EO, Chow HM, Nieto-Castañón A, Tourville JA, Guenther FH, Chang SE. Anomalous morphology in left hemisphere motor and premotor cortex of children who stutter. Brain. 2018;141(9):2670-84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy199 PMid:30084910.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy199
  • 27
    Connally EL, Ward D, Pliatsikas C, Finnegan S, Jenkinson M, Boyles R, et al. Separation of trait and state in stuttering. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(8):3109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24063 PMid:29624772.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24063
  • 28
    Erdemir A, Walden TA, Jefferson CM, Choi D, Jones RM. The effect of emotion on articulation rate in persistence and recovery of childhood stuttering. J Fluency Disord. 2018;56:1-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.11.003 PMid:29443691.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.11.003
  • 29
    Wolk L, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Coexistence of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(5):906-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.906 PMid:8246479.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.906
  • 30
    Caruso AJ, Abbs JH, Gracco VL. Kinematic analysis of multiple movement coordination during speech in stutterers. Brain. 1988;111(2):439-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/111.2.439 PMid:3378144.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/111.2.439

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    03 Apr 2020
  • Date of issue
    2020

History

  • Received
    09 Jan 2019
  • Accepted
    12 June 2019
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br