Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Gross motor performance and its association with neonatal and familial factors and day care exposure among children up to three years old

Abstracts

OBJECTIVE: To analyze gross motor performance and its association with neonatal and familial factors and day care exposure among children up to three years of age attending public day care centers. METHODS:This was a cross-sectional study that evaluated 145 children (58 aged 6-11 months, 54 aged 12-23 months and 33 aged 24-38 months) attending six public day care centers in Piracicaba, State of São Paulo. The Peabody Developmental Motor Scale-2 test was used to assess overall gross motor performance and the motor subtests that make up the scale (reflexes, stationary skills, locomotion skills and object manipulation). Neonatal, familial and daycare center exposure data were also collected and their association with suspected delays in gross motor performance was investigated. RESULTS:The prevalence of suspected delays in gross motor performance was 17%, with disadvantages among children under 24 months of age and with regard to locomotion skills. There were associations of risk between suspected delays in gross motor performance and family income, and between suspected delays in locomotion skills and parental educational level. Children whose families had a monthly income of up to 700 reais were 2.81 times more likely to present delays in gross motor performance. Children whose parents had up to eight years of education were 4.63 times more likely to present delays in locomotion skills. There was no association of risk with the other variables. CONCLUSION: The results indicate the need for greater attention to motor development during the first 24 months of children who attend day care centers, especially those with low-income and less-educated parents.

day care centers; child; child development; environment; risk factors


OBJETIVO: Analisar o desempenho motor grosso e sua associação com fatores neonatais, familiares e de exposição à creche em crianças com até três anos de idade, frequentadoras de creches públicas. MÉTODOS: Estudo transversal no qual foram avaliadas 145 crianças (58 com idade entre 6-11 meses, 54 entre 12-23 meses e 33 entre 24-38 meses) frequentadoras de seis creches públicas de Piracicaba (SP). O teste Peabody Developmental Motor Scale-2 foi utilizado para avaliação do desempenho motor grosso global e subtestes motores que compõem a escala (Reflexos, Habilidades Estacionárias, Habilidades de Locomoção e Manipulação de Objetos). Foram coletados dados neonatais, familiares e de exposição à creche e pesquisada a associação desses ao desempenho motor suspeito de atraso. RESULTADOS:A prevalência de suspeita de atraso no desempenho motor grosso foi de 17%, com desvantagens em crianças menores de 24 meses e em Habilidades de Locomoção; encontrada associação de risco de desempenho motor grosso suspeito de atraso e renda familiar, e suspeita de atraso em Habilidades de Locomoção e escolaridade paterna. Crianças cujas famílias tinham renda mensal até R$700,00 estavam 2,81 vezes mais expostas a apresentar desempenho motor grosso suspeito de atraso. Crianças cujos pais tinham até oito anos de escolaridade apresentaram risco 4,63 vezes maior de atraso em Habilidades de Locomoção. Não foi encontrada associação de risco com as demais variáveis. CONCLUSÃO:Os resultados apontam maior atenção ao desenvolvimento motor durante os primeiros 24 meses de crianças que frequentam creches, especialmente as inseridas em famílias com menor renda mensal e cujos pais têm menos escolaridade.

creches; criança; desenvolvimento infantil; meio ambiente; fatores de risco


ORIGINAL ARTICLES


Gross motor performance and its association with neonatal and familial factors and day care exposure among children up to three years old

Santos DCCI, IV; Tolocka REII; Carvalho JI; Heringer LRCI, III; Almeida CMI; Miquelote AFIV

IGraduate Program in Physical Therapy, Faculdade de Ciências da Saúde (FACIS), Universidade Metodista de Piracicaba (UNIMEP), Piracicaba (SP), Brazil

IIGraduate Program in Physical Education, FACIS, UNIMEP

IIICentro Universitário do Leste de Minas Gerais (UNILESTE), Ipatinga (MG), Brazil

IVUndergraduate Program in Physical Therapy, FACIS, UNIMEP

Correspondence to

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To analyze gross motor performance and its association with neonatal and familial factors and day care exposure among children up to three years of age attending public day care centers.

METHODS: This was a cross-sectional study that evaluated 145 children (58 aged 6-11 months, 54 aged 12-23 months and 33 aged 24-38 months) attending six public day care centers in Piracicaba, State of São Paulo. The Peabody Developmental Motor Scale-2 test was used to assess overall gross motor performance and the motor subtests that make up the scale (reflexes, stationary skills, locomotion skills and object manipulation). Neonatal, familial and daycare center exposure data were also collected and their association with suspected delays in gross motor performance was investigated.

RESULTS: The prevalence of suspected delays in gross motor performance was 17%, with disadvantages among children under 24 months of age and with regard to locomotion skills. There were associations of risk between suspected delays in gross motor performance and family income, and between suspected delays in locomotion skills and parental educational level. Children whose families had a monthly income of up to 700 reais were 2.81 times more likely to present delays in gross motor performance. Children whose parents had up to eight years of education were 4.63 times more likely to present delays in locomotion skills. There was no association of risk with the other variables.

CONCLUSION: The results indicate the need for greater attention to motor development during the first 24 months of children who attend day care centers, especially those with low-income and less-educated parents.

Key words: day care centers; child; child development; environment; risk factors.

Introduction

Child development consists in various interdependent domains (sensory-motor, cognitive and socioemotional) influenced by biological factors (i.e. gestational age, birth weight, etc.), socioenvironmental factors (e.g. socioeconomic status, parental education, etc) and hereditary factors; all of these factors can be affected by adverse or favorable situations1. The first years of life are particularly important as that is when vital development occurs in all domains due to intense events in the development of the nervous system (NS), modulated by the quality of the environment1. In the general context of child development, motor development is regarded as an essential component with implications to other developmental aspects2. During the first years, it provides the foundation for subsequent development and optimizes functional performance in areas such as learning, self-care, leisure, and games3. It is also an important indicator of NS maturity and integrity and of general well-being4.

In recent decades, the growing number of children enrolled in day care centers5 in the first few months of life5 has introduced a new, permeating factor into their development, namely the daily experience of a collective environment. According to the United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF)6, in 2001, 10.7% of the Brazilian children up to three years old attended day care centers; in 2006, this proportion had risen to 15.5%; it is estimated that by 2011 this proportion will reach 50%. Although access to childhood education is a fundamental right6,7 and its purpose is the child's integral development, the increase in the proportion of children who experience this collective environment raises some concerns. Belsky8 warns that there are risks and benefits associated with the daily care dispensed in early childhood; thus, it is important to know its effect on child development, without losing sight of the various factors that could potentially influence this development. In Brazil, studies on children in day care centers are scarce and indicate problems concerning employee qualifications; infrastructure; the adoption of routines with emphasis on nourishment and hygiene; increased exposure to infections and possible repercussions for motor development9-13.

It is estimated that around 200 million children up to the age of five who live in developing countries fail to reach their cognitive, motor, and socioemotional development potential, considering that this is the result of the interrelation of genetic, biological, and socioenvironmental factors1. A better knowledge of the impact of these factors on child development in their first years of life is fundamental to establishing strategies to promote progress and prevent delays. In this context, the aim of the present study was to analyze gross motor performance and its association with neonatal and familial factors, as well as day care exposure of children up to three years old who attend public day care centers.

Methods

This was an exploratory cross-sectional study in which 145 children aged 0-3 attending six public day care centers in Piracicaba (SP) were evaluated as to motor performance, neonatal and familial characteristics, and day care exposure. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee (Protocol 80/05) of Universidade Metodista de Piracicaba (UNIMEP). For the sample calculation, the following parameters were taken into account: a finite population, 1456 children enrolled in the 1st, 2nd, and 3rd stages of preschool (year of 2006 – Municipal Education Department); estimated 20% probability of finding changes; 7% sampling error with a confidence interval of 95%; estimated minimal sample of 115 children. A convenience sample from the public day care centers of Piracicaba (SP) was used considering the following criteria: no intervention of any kind (e.g. physical education); interest on the part of the principals; and proximity to the university where the study was based. Three day care centers were located in the east part, two in the south part, and one in the north part of town. The collection was carried out on location from March 2006 to November 2007. The study included 0-3 year olds who attended day care centers on a full-time basis and whose guardians signed a consent form. Exclusion criteria were malformations, neurological problems, genetic syndromes and severe illnesses. From an initial sample of 162 children, 17 were excluded for absence due to illness or for leaving the school. Thus, 145 children (73 boys and 72 girls) composed the final sample of this study, of which 58 were 6-11 months old, 54 were 12-23 months old, and 33 were 24-38 months old.

The Peabody Developmental Motor Scale-2 (PDMS-2) test14 was used to obtain the results of the overall gross motor performance and the motor subtests of this scale: Reflexes (up to 11 months) – evaluates the ability to automatically react to environmental stimulus (e.g. walking reflex, protective reactions); Stationary Skills (1-71 months) – measures the ability to adjust the body to the center of gravity and maintain balance (e.g. control of the head, trunk, and orthostatic position); Locomotion Skills (1-71 months) – evaluates the ability to move (e.g., to roll over, walk, crawl, etc.); Object Manipulation (12-71 months) – tests the ability to throw, catch and kick a ball). The performance in the gross motor scale is expressed by means of the Gross Motor Quotient (GMQ), with a mean of 100 and standard deviation (SD) of 15. In each subtest, it is expressed by the Standard Score (SS), with a mean of 10 and SD of 03; the performance can be rated as: very high, high, above average, average, below average, poor, very poor. In the present study, the motor performance variable was treated as a dichotomous variable; adequate gross motor performance when GMQ>90 and suspected delay when GMQ<90; adequate performance in each subtest when EP>8 and suspected delay when EP<8; in other words, there was suspected delay when the performance was below average, poor, or very poor.

The PDMS-2 was chosen for being a standardized instrument that evaluates gross motricity in detail and for being a diagnosis scale. It is also suitable to the age group studied and can be applied to day care centers. Each child was evaluated, according to chronological age or age corrected for prematurity, by two examiners (a graduate student and an undergraduate research fellow). One was responsible for conducting the test, and the other for recording the results. The examiners attended PDMS-2 training sessions and took part in an inter-examiner agreement study that included 14 infants evaluated separately by each examiner (Intraclass Correlation Coefficient =0.83; p<0.001). The resources used for the tests included a mat, a table and chair, PDMS-2 original material, and toys. The child had to be alert and wear clothes that did not restrict movements. The test was scheduled according to the daily routine of the day care centers, respecting the allotted times for meals, bathing, and napping. If interrupted, the tests were completed within five days, as established by the scale14.

The following independent neonatal variables were considered: gestational age (< or > 37 weeks of gestation) and birth weight (< or > 2500 g.; World Health Organization – CID1015) and the Apgar score (< or > seven in the 1st and 5th minute of life). To collect the neonatal data, the family was asked to provide the researchers with a photocopy of the child's Health Record Book; the missing information was completed by consulting the maternity records. Family data were collected by means of a closed questionnaire, adapted from the items on familial characteristics of the Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development - Self-Report16, taking into account: parental age (< or > 20 years); parental education level (< or > eight years); parental cohabitation (yes or no); number of adults (> or < two) and children (< or > two) in the same household; monthly family income (median income < or > R$700.00, i.e. approximately twice the minimum wage). For risk analysis, we also considered the child's median time attending day care centers (< or > 6 months) and the child's mean age at the time of enrollment (< or > 7 months).

Statistical analysis

The continuous variables were expressed by central tendency and dispersion measures, and the categorical variables by frequencies. To test the differences in motor performance between the groups (defined by age), the ANOVA test for independent groups was used (the assumptions of normal distribution [Kolmogorov-Smirnov test] and homogeneity of variance [Levene's test] were confirmed); as a post hoc, Bonferroni's test was applied. The chi-square test was used to analyze the association between the proportion of participants in the categories of the variable motor performance and the age groups and motor subtests. For the association and risk analysis of categorical data, we used the Prevalence Ratio Index or Odds Ratio (OR), with the respective Confidence Interval (CI). The risk association was considered significant when the lower limit of the CI was greater than one. The level of significance was set at 5%.

Results

Overall, the mean performance of the group was compatible with the reference sample for the GMQ (100±11.02). However, considering the mean of performances according to the age groups (6-11 months = 97.15±9.03; 12-23 months = 99.42±11.63; 24-38 months = 105±11.18) there was a difference in the monthly mean score (F=7.46; p=0.01). The post hoc test indicated that children older than 24 months had a better performance, when compared to those aged between 6-11 and 12-23 months.

The prevalence of suspected delay in motor performance is displayed in Table 1. There was suspected delay in the overall gross motor performance in 17% of the children. Considering each age group, there is a tendency to a higher prevalence of suspected delay among children up to 11 months old (17.2%) and between 12-23 months (22%), compared with those older than 24 months (6%), although there was no statistical significance (x2=3.91; p=0.141). Subtest analysis shows a higher prevalence of suspected delay in locomotion skills, followed by reflexes, object manipulation and stationary skills, with a significant difference between the motor subtests (x2=13.30; p=0.004).

Because the prevalence of suspected delay in locomotion skills (16%) was similar to that of overall gross motor performance (17%), we also investigated the risk association for this subtest (Table 2). Children from families with an income of up to R$700.00 were 2.81 times more likely to have suspected delay in gross motor performance. Children whose parents had up to eight years of education were 4.63 times more likely to have suspected delay in locomotion skills. No risk association was found between the delay in motor performance and the other variables studied.

Discussion

Considering the group as a whole, 17% of the children had suspected delay in gross motor performance. The motor performance analysis showed disadvantages in children under 24 months old and in locomotion skills, as well as a risk association between suspected delay in gross motor performance and family income and between suspected delay in locomotion skills and parental education level.

Several studies on children who attend day care centers and preschools report suspected developmental delays, including motor development. However, the studies are still inconclusive, and the prevalence of delay or suspected delay ranges from 10% to 43% in the different studies11-13,17,18. The suspected delays found in the present study are also variable if one takes into account the values for different ages (analysis of variance) and for the different motor domains (Table 1). Among the studies identified in the literature, the present study is the only one to use a scale of diagnosis to assess the motor development of children who attend day care centers, therefore we believe the results may contribute to understanding the heterogeneity of the findings.

The disadvantage in locomotion skills evidenced by the present study suggests this is the most affected motor domain in the group examined, especially in the first two years of life, a period considered fundamental for the acquisition of independent erect locomotion2. There is evidence that, in many institutions, infants spend most of the day in their cribs, which limits the opportunities to explore the environment and interact with other children, with potential repercussions for locomotion skills5. According to Campos et al.19, early locomotion experiences are essential to developmental changes, with a significant impact on social and emotional development, communication through motor actions, spatial perception and cognition.

The dynamism of the developmental process and the greater vulnerability associated with young age may explain the differences found in the various age groups. The difference in motor performance between children under 24 months of age and children above that age suggests that the potential for full development is optimized with locomotion independence, i.e. the characteristics that influence the way a child moves change over time according to experience and learning, but also according to context and task20. One of the most common models used to explain the changes in child development is the transactional model which states that there is a degree of plasticity inherent to the child and to the environment21. The basic assumption of this model is that development is facilitated by a bidirectional and reciprocal interaction between the child and their environment, which are actively engaged with one another, changing and being changed by this interaction. Development is considered unique and peculiar, resulting in the balance between the risk factors and the protection factors of the child and the context21.

Bradley and Vandell22 point out that day care experiences interact with experiences at home and their own characteristics to produce the results in development. The risk associations that were found suggest the preponderant influence of the family on the motor development of the group studied. According to Belsky et al.23, the family is the strongest and most consistent predictor of child development, but the quality of the care given in the first few years is very important.

Although the influence of neonatal risk factors is well described in the literature, the present study had a low occurrence of preterm births with low weight and Apgar score below seven (Table 2). In addition, no neonatal clinic complications were found, and preterm children were evaluated based on the corrected age. There is evidence that, in the absence of other disturbances and with age correction of preterm children, the motor development may be similar to that of children born at term24.

There was also a low occurrence of potentially unfavorable family situations (e.g. teenage parents, low maternal education level). This may at least partially explain the absence of risk association of suspected delay for these variables25. As for the risk associations that were identified, they suggest greater risk of overall delay among children from low income families and a risk of delay in locomotion skills associated with low paternal education level. The relationship between low socioeconomic status/low income and developmental disadvantage is well known1,26-28. Poverty amplifies children's biological vulnerability, leading to unfavorable results in development27. The underprivileged segment of society combines social, economic and biological factors that determine a greater chance of developmental delay28. Poverty and its associated problems (i.e. malnutrition, unsanitary living conditions; parental education and inadequate stimulation at home) are regarded as the main risk factors in child development1 and may have contributed to the findings.

The present study showed that, even in a group of children/families who use the public education system, there is a greater disadvantage (higher risk of motor development delay) associated with lower income (46% with an income under R$700.00; Table 2). Regarding parental education level, this result comes at a time of debate about the pertinence of including the father and his possible influence on child development. The education level of the head of the household substantially affects child well-being due to the vulnerability of children in early childhood29. It also has indirect repercussions on proper child rearing because it influences the access to and comprehension of the information disseminated by health and education services and by the media30. The father's role in neurological and psychomotor development was evidenced in two studies. While studying the cognitive and motor development of 788 Chilean infants, researchers found that the best combination of risk categories for child development were temperament, maternal intelligence, paternal role and stimulation at home27. The study by Barros et al.11 showed that biologically healthy children may suffer negative influences from environmental risk factors, including the absence of a father11.

The present study did not find any risk associations between age at enrollment in day care or time of day care exposure and motor development, however the quality of the care received by the child may at least partially explain the findings in the literature11-13,17,18. Surveys on the quality of education in Brazilian preschools (1996-2003) showed that educators had difficulty overcoming poor eating and hygiene habits to promote practices that lead to full development9. Although preschools aim to foster development, the quality of the day care center environment as a place for child development has been questioned30,31. The findings of the present study and the reports in the literature suggest that, even if day care centers do not impair child development, they may not be acting satisfactorily to support and promote it, as recommended by the National Education Guidelines7 and the National Curriculum Reference Guide5.

The limitations of the present study were its cross-sectional design; the lack of investigations into the quality of the service provided at each day care center and at home, and the use of an assessment instrument still not validated in Brazil, which is also a limitation of this field of knowledge. There is a lack of instruments developed and/or validated in Brazil to evaluate early motor development. As a consequence, the use of foreign instruments has been frequent in Brazilian research4,24,26.

In general, the results of the present study emphasize the need for more attention to motor development during the first 24 months of life of preschool children, particularly those from lower income and less educated families. Given the multicausal nature of development, which is influenced by child/environment interaction, and the importance of the object of study, i.e. child development, further investigation is needed into the associations found in the present study and into the use of study models (e.g. longitudinal studies) to explore the cause-and-effect relationships.

Acknowledgements

UNIMEP Research Support Fund (Protocol 369/05); UNIMEP Extension Support Fund (Protocol 21/06).

References

  • 1. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60-70.
  • 2. Wijnhoven TMA, de Onis M, Onyang AW, Wang T, Bjoerneboe GA, Bhandari N, et al. Assessment of gross motor development in the WHO multicentre growth reference study. Food Nutr Bull. 2004;25(1Suppl):S37-45.
  • 3. Goyen TA, Lui K. Longitudinal motor development of "apparently normal" high-risk infants at 18 months, 3 and 5 years. Early Hum Dev. 2002;70(1-2):103-15.
  • 4. Santos DC, Gabbard C, Gonçalves VM. Motor development during the first year: a comparative study. J Genet Psychol. 2001;162(2):143-53.
  • 5. Ministério da Educação e do Desporto. Secretaria de Educação Fundamental. Referencial curricular nacional para a educação infantil - vol.1 (livro na internet). Brasília-DF; 1998 [acesso em 2008 jul 25]. Disponível em: http://portal.mec.gov.br/seb/arquivos/pdf/rcnei_vol1.pdf
  • 6
    Unicef. Situação Mundial da Infância 2008 - Caderno Brasil [livro na internet]. Brasil; 2008 [acesso em 2008 jul 25]. Disponível em: http://www.unicef.org/brazil/pt/resources_11319.htm
  • 7
    Presidência da República - Casa Civil. Lei de Diretrizes e Bases da Educação Nacional - LDBEN. Lei nº 9.304, de 20 de dezembro de 1996. [acesso em 2006 Nov 22]. Disponível em: http://www.planalto.gov.br/CCIVIL_03/LEIS/L9394.htm
  • 8. Belsky J. Early child care and early child development: major findings of the NICHD study of early child care. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2006;3(1):95-110.
  • 9. Campos MM, Füllgraf J, Wiggers V. A qualidade da educação infantil brasileira: alguns resultados de pesquisa. Cad Pesqui. 2006;36(127):87-128.
  • 10. Nesti MM, Goldbaum M. Infectious diseases and daycare and preschool education. J Pediatr. 2007;83(6):299-312.
  • 11. de Barros KM, Fragoso AG, de Oliveira AL, Cabral Filho JE, de Castro RM. Do environmental influences alter motor abilities acquisition? A comparison among children from day-care centers and private schools. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003;61(2A):170-5.
  • 12. Souza SC. Avaliação do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor do pré-escolar de creches públicas de Cuiabá-MT [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2004.
  • 13. Biscegli1 TS, Polis LB, Santos LM, Vicentin M. Avaliação do estado nutricional e do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor em crianças freqüentadoras de creche. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2007;25(4):337-42.
  • 14. Folio MR, Fewell RR. Peabody Developmental Motor Scales. 2Ş ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2000.
  • 15. OMS - Organização Mundial da Saúde. Definições. In: CID-10 - Classificação estatística internacional de doenças e problemas relacionados a saúde. 10Ş revisão. v.1, tradução do Centro Colaborador da OMS para a classificação de doenças em português. 7Ş ed. São Paulo: EDUSP; 1999. p. 1181-6.
  • 16. Rodrigues LPLBA. Development and validation of the AHEMD-SR [tese]. Texas: Texas A&M University; 2005.
  • 17. Caon G, Ries LGK. Suspeita de atraso no desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor em idade precoce: uma abordagem em creches públicas. Temas Desenvolv. 2003;12(70):11-7.
  • 18. Rezende MA, Beteli VC, Santos JLF. Follow-up of the child's motor abilities in day-care centers and pre-schools. Rev Latino-am Enfermagem. 2005;13(5):619-25.
  • 19. Campos JJ, Anderson DI, Barbu-Roth MA, Hubbard ED, Hertenstein MJ, Witherington D. Travel broadens the mind. Infancy. 2000;1(2):149-219.
  • 20. Gagen LM, Getchell N. Using 'constraints' to design developmentally appropriate movement activities for early childhood education. Early Childhood Educ J. 2006;34(3):227-32.
  • 21. Sameroff AJ, Fiese BH. Transactional regulation: the developmental ecology of early intervention. In: Shokoff JP, Meisels SJ, editores. Handbook of early childhood intervention. 2Ş ed. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2000. p.135-59.
  • 22. Bradley RH, Vandell DL. Child care and the well-being of children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(7):669-76.
  • 23. Belsky J, Vandell DL, Burhinal M, Clarke-Stewart KA, McCartney K, Owen MT. Are there long-term effects of early child care? Child Dev. 2007;78(2):681-701.
  • 24. Mancini MC, Teixeira S, Araújo LG, Paixão ML, Magalhães LC, Coelho ZAC, et al. Estudo do desenvolvimento da função motora aos 8 e 12 meses de idade em crianças nascidas pré-termo e a termo. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2002;60(4):974-80.
  • 25. Kappel MDB, Carvalho MC, Kramer S. Perfil das crianças de 0 a 6 anos que freqüentam creches, pré-escolas e escolas: uma análise dos resultados da pesquisa sobre padrões de vida/IBGE. Rev Bras Educ. 2001;16:35-47.
  • 26. Lima MC, Eickmann SH, Lima AC, Guerra MQ, Lira PI, Huttly SR, et al. Determinants of mental and motor development at 12 months in a low income population: a cohort study in northeast Brazil. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93(7):969-75.
  • 27. de Andraca I, Pino P, de la Parra A, Rivera F, Castillo M. Risk factors for psychomotor development among infants born under optimal biological conditions. Rev Saude Publica. 1998;32(2):138-47.
  • 28. Halpern R, Giugliani ERJ, Victora CG, Barros FC, Horta BL. Fatores de risco para suspeita de atraso no desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor aos 12 meses de vida. J Pediatr. 2000;76(6):421-8.
  • 29
    Unicef. Situação da criança brasileira - Índice de Desenvolvimento Infantil [livro na Internet]. Brasil; 2006 [acesso em 2008 jul 25]. Disponível em: http://www.unicef.org/brazil/pt/Pags_108_123_IDI_Abre.pdf
  • 30. Moreira LVC, Lordelo ER. Creche em ambiente urbano: ressonâncias no ecossistema desenvolvimental. Interação Psicol. 2002;6(1):19-30.
  • 31. Amorim KS, Vitoria T, Rossetti-Ferreira MC. Rede de significações: perspectivas para análise da inserção de bebês na creche. Cad Pesqui. 2000;109:115-44.
  • Correspondência para:
    Denise Castilho Cabrera Santos
    Programa de Pós-Graduação em Fisioterapia - Bloco 7 sala 33
    Faculdade de Ciência da Saúde, FACIS/UNIMEP
    Rodovia do Açúcar Km 156
    CEP 13400-911, Piracicaba (SP), Brasil
    e-mail:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      17 Apr 2009
    • Date of issue
      Apr 2009

    History

    • Reviewed
      30 Nov 2008
    • Received
      07 Aug 2008
    • Accepted
      13 Jan 2009
    Associação Brasileira de Pesquisa e Pós-Graduação em Fisioterapia Rod. Washington Luís, Km 235, Caixa Postal 676, CEP 13565-905 - São Carlos, SP - Brasil, Tel./Fax: 55 16 3351 8755 - São Carlos - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: contato@rbf-bjpt.org.br