Open-access Association between gestational age and birth weight on the language development of Brazilian children: a systematic review

Abstracts

OBJECTIVE:  To review the literature that addresses the relationship between prematurity, birth weight, and development of language in Brazilian children.

SOURCES:  A systematic review of studies published between 2003 and 2012 in English and Portuguese and indexed in PubMed, LILACS, and SciELO. The following key words were used in the searches: Prematuro, Prematuridade, Linguagem, Prematurity, Language, Speech-Language Pathology. Fifty-seven articles were retrieved, 13 of which were included in the systematic review.

SUMMARY OF THE FINDINGS:  The results showed an association between prematurity, low birth weight, and language development. In studies that made comparisons between preterm and term infants, there was evidence that preterm infants had poorer performance on indicators of language. It was also observed that children born with lower birth weight had a poorer performance on measures of language when compared to children with higher weight and closer to 37 weeks of gestational age. Regarding the type of language assessed, expression proved to be more impaired than reception. Higher parental education and family income were indicated as protective factors for the development of language. Conversely, lower birth weight and higher degree of prematurity emerged as risk factors.

CONCLUSIONS:  Preterm birth and low birth weight poses risks for the language development of children, especially in the first years of life. Therefore, it is essential that pediatricians are aware of the language development of these children to ensure proper treatment.

Infant prematurity; Low birth weight; Child; Language; Language development disorders; Language development


OBJETIVO:  Revisar a literatura que aborda a relação entre prematuridade, peso ao nascimento e desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças brasileiras.

FONTES DOS DADOS:  Revisão sistemática de estudos publicados entre 2003 e 2012, nos idiomas inglês e português, e indexados nas bases de dados PubMed, Lilacs e SciELO. Nas buscas foram usadas as palavras-chave: Prematuro, Prematuridade, Linguagem, Prematurity, Language, Speech-Language Pathology. Foram identificados 57 artigos, dos quais 13 foram incluídos na revisão sistemática.

SÍNTESE DOS DADOS:  Os resultados apontaram uma associação entre prematuridade, baixo peso ao nascer e desenvolvimento de linguagem. Nos estudos que fizeram comparações entre crianças nascidas pré-termo e crianças nascidas a termo, houve evidências de que os prematuros apresentaram pior desempenho nos indicadores de linguagem. Também foi observado que as crianças nascidas com menor peso ao nascer obtiveram pior desempenho nas avaliações de linguagem quando comparadas com as crianças com maior peso e idade gestacional mais próxima de 37 semanas. Em relação ao tipo de linguagem avaliado, a expressiva mostrou-se mais prejudicada do que a receptiva. Como fatores de proteção ao desenvolvimento de linguagem foram apontados a maior escolaridade dos pais e a maior renda familiar. E, em contrapartida, o menor peso ao nascimento e o maior grau de prematuridade surgiram como fatores de risco.

CONCLUSÕES:  A prematuridade e o baixo peso ao nascer representam riscos para o desenvolvimento linguístico das crianças, especialmente nos primeiros anos de vida. Sendo assim, torna-se fundamental que os pediatras estejam atentos ao desenvolvimento da linguagem dessas crianças para tratamento adequado.

Prematuridade; Baixo peso ao nascimento; Criança; Linguagem; Atraso de desenvolvimento de linguagem; Desenvolvimento de linguagem


Introduction

In Brazil, in 2011, 285,592 infants were born preterm and 248,217 had low birth weight, corresponding to 9.80% and 8.52% of total births (2,913,160), respectively.1 Gestational age (GA) and birth weight (BW) have been identified as important biological risk factors for alterations in children's language development.2 , 3 and 4

The literature in the area of neonatology indicates that although technological and scientific developments have contributed significantly to the decrease in mortality among preterm infants, prematurity and low BW still appear as causes of neonatal mortality and morbidity, showing a strong clinical and epidemiological impact.5Therefore, the follow-up of this group is important to health promotion.

Studies have shown that the lower the GA and the BW, the greater is the risk of developmental disorders.6 The risks of developmental alterations in these children are higher not only because they are more susceptible to diseases, but also because they are exposed to iatrogenic factors, such time away from the mother, prolonged time in the incubator, drug effects, mechanical ventilation, and stress due to long-term manipulation.7 Thus, newborns who resist perinatal complications become prone to manifest alterations in their development and may have neurological, sensory, and language deficits.8

The literature has shown a significant delay in language development of children born preterm and with low BW,9 and the early recognition of developmental disorders can help to include these children in specific intervention programs, thus minimizing the risk of irreversible dysfunctions and improving their quality of life.7 and 10 Therefore, this study aimed to systematically review research on the association between prematurity, low BW, and language development in Brazilian children.

Methods

Research strategy

A systematic review was carried out in the PubMed, LILACS, and SciELO databases, using combinations of the following keywords: prematuro linguagem, prematuridade linguagem, prematurity language, speech-language pathology (always including the word AND). The keywords were selected by researching MeSH (PubMed) and DeCS terms (LILACS and SciELO).

Selection criteria

The criteria used for study inclusion were: original articles that established an association between GA, BW, and language development in Brazilian children; studies published in the last ten years (January/2003 to December/2012) in Portuguese and English, and that used the cohort, case-control, longitudinal, cross-sectional, descriptive analytical, and retrospective methods.

The dependent variables used in this review were the variables obtained as the result of standardized tests (receptive language and/or expressive language). The independent variables were GA, BW, gender, age at time of evaluation, family income, and maternal level of education.

For the purposes of the present study, prematurity was considered at three levels: borderline preterm (GA 35-36 weeks), moderately preterm (GA 31-34 weeks), and extremely preterm (GA ≤ 30 weeks). Newborns with low BW were classified as low BW (< 2,500 g), very low birth weight (< 1,500 g), and extremely low BW (< 1,000 g).11

Data analysis

The initial search resulted in a total of 57 articles, of which 36 were excluded after the title and abstract were read, as they did not address the subject, were literature reviews, or were case reports. Of the remaining 21 articles, eight were excluded due to duplication. The 13 selected articles were read and included in the review. The results of the assessed studies were analyzed descriptively.

Results and discussion

The characteristics of the studies, such as the methodology used, age of the assessed children, and sample size are described in Table 1. Most of the studies assessed children younger than 2 years and sample size varied. The years 2009 and 2012 featured the greatest number of articles published on the subject.

Table 1
Description of studies on language development in preterm children.

Table 2 describes the language assessment tools used in the studies, demonstrating a variety of tests, scales, and lists, among techniques used by the authors.

Table 2
Assessment tools used in the studies.

Regarding the locations where studies were conducted, eleven were performed in the Southeast Region, whereas the South and Northeast Regions were represented by one study each.2 , 8 , 9 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 , 19 , 20 and 21 It was not possible to make comparisons between the results obtained in these regions due to the difference in the number of studies carried out by region, which is indicative of the need to perform studies in other Brazilian regions.

Language skills of premature infants were evaluated in two different manners by the studies: those that focused on expressive language2 and 15 and those that addressed both receptive and expressive language.8 , 9 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 16 , 17 , 18 , 19 , 20 and 21 The two studies that evaluated expressive language only2 and 15 found a delay in language development in preterm children when compared to children born at term, and one found a significantly lower difference in the vocabulary in all the semantic categories.15

In the 11 studies that analyzed the receptive and expressive language, four found significant differences in both types of language,9 , 12 , 13 and 17 three found differences in expressive language only,8 , 14 and 19 two did not differentiate the type of language but demonstrated significant difference,16 and 20 and two found no difference in any of the language types.18 and 21

Among those that found significant differences in both types of language, the difference found in expressive language was more significant when compared to receptive language.12 , 13 and 17 In one study that showed a significant difference,8 the most delayed expressive language item in premature infants was first word production (p = 0.0096). In a study that showed no difference between the full-term and preterm children,18 a possible explanation reported by the authors was the assistance parents received in the follow-up carried out at the service, which included information on how to stimulate the child's language. Therefore, the review suggests that expressive language was more affected than receptive language in premature infants.

The language acquisition delay in these children may be associated with brain injuries related to neonatal complications,8 and 22 with central nervous system immaturity,8 , 22 and 23 and the child's interaction with the environment and with people.24 The maturational processes and the individual's interactions with the environment are influenced by organic, psychological, social, and economic conditions that may have a negative or positive impact on child development.25Therefore, the interaction between parent and child becomes of utmost importance for child development, especially in preterm infants. Thus, it is very important that pediatricians and speech therapists are aware of all these aspects when observing the language of premature infants, so that, if necessary, early referrals and interventions can be performed.

In the six studies that compared language development between groups differentiated by GA or BW,9 , 13 , 15 , 16 , 17 and 21 it was observed that children born with lower GA and BW showed the worst results in language assessments. Most studies that performed comparisons between groups differentiated by GA found that samples of preterm children had poorer performance on language assessment tests when compared to children born at term, and the lower GA was associated with children's lower phrasal extent (p = 0.016),15 losses in the expressive language area at 4-6 months and 10-12 months of age,13 and losses in the expressive language area in children aged 12-36 months.21

In the study by Schirmer et al.,9 which used Denver II, Bayley II, and the Developmental Sequences of Language Behavior by Nicolosi for evaluation, it was observed that the mental, psychomotor, and behavioral development in newborns with BW < 1,500 g showed a statistically significant association with language development, whereas in infants with BW > 1,500 g, this association remained significant in the mental aspect.9 Thus, the language development delay may be associated with delays in other areas of development, especially in infants with lower BW.

In the three studies that compared language assessment results with BW,15 , 17 and 21 lower BW was associated with the worse performance in expressive language,21lower number of words (p = 0.045), and lower phrasal extension (p = 0.019).15 When comparing low BW with very-low BW preterm infants at age 12-24 months, infants with very low BW showed higher losses in the assessed areas compared to those with low weight.17

Oliveira et al. 8 compared full-term newborns small for GA (FTNB/SGA) with preterm newborns adequate for GA (PTNB/AGA) using the Early Language Milestone (ELM) scale. Both groups had equivalent linguistic production for chronological age up to 6 months. This result differs from that found by Pereira and Funayama,13 who found differences in language between the groups at the ages of 0-3 months, even those with corrected ages. The items considered in the assessments of both studies are different at the age of up to 6 months; while the ELM scale considers smiling and lip vibration the first manifestations of language, the assessment protocol used by Pereira and Funayama13only considers vowel emissions. In the 9th month, the items assessed in both tools are more similar and the results found in the two studies indicated a lower production of babbling, although the ELM scale showed that these findings also occurred in full-term AGA children. Therefore, it is difficult to compare results in language studies because of the different instruments used for assessment.

Regarding the age at which premature infants showed a more meaningful language delay, three studies followed children longitudinally and were able to analyze this aspect.8 , 13 and 16 Oliveira et al. 8 evaluated the expressive language in two groups of children, FTNB/SGA and PTNB/AGA, at 6, 9, 12, and 18 months and found that until the 6th month, the infants showed a normal performance for their age, and only in the 12th month was there a statistically significant delay in the FTNB/SGA group, which remained with polysyllabic babbling, and in the PTNB/AGA group, in the production of first words. Pereira and Funayama13 reviewed children's language at five different levels: I (0-3 months), II (4-6 months), III (7-9 months), IV (10-12 months), and V (13-15 months).

As for receptive language, preterm children had a significantly worse performance at level I, and in expressive language, performance was worse in levels II and IV. Based on these results, children up to 12 months of age were more likely to have language development delays. Lamônica and Picolini16 observed children's language in two periods, 6-12 months and 12-24 months, and although in both periods the results were significantly worse for preterm infants, more children had language delay in the period of 6-12 months than at 12-24 months. Thus, studies have shown an evolution of preterm infants during the first two years of life, with language development drawing nearer to the development of children born at term. This development is known as the "catch-up phenomenon," which may occur with both preterm and low BW newborns.24

Another aspect discussed in the articles was whether gender influenced language development.8 , 15 , 19 and 20 In the four analyzed studies, two of them found no statistically significant difference,8 and 15 whereas the other two indicated that male gender was a risk factor for alterations in language development.19 and 20

In a systematic review of literature carried out in national and international databases between January 2005 and June 2010, one of the aspects analyzed in preterm children was language.26 A total of ten articles were associated with language, and although many did not include Brazilian children, a comparison might be relevant. Vieira and Linhares26identified in some studies that male gender was a risk predictor for language delay in premature infants. There is still no consensus among the results, but there is an indication that male children are at increased risk for the language development delay.

Language acquisition and development involve multiple aspects, and in addition to the physical and biological factors, it is important to consider social and cultural factors that involve variables such as the environment where the child lives, the quality of adult-child interaction,27 , 28 and 29 and the degree of parental education and family income. In this review, one study assessed maternal education and per capitaincome 21 and two analyzed parental education and family income. 15 and 26 In one study, poorer performance in receptive language was correlated with low maternal educational level. 21 In the review by Vieira and Linhares, 26 low educational level was considered a risk factor for language development, whereas higher family income was a protective factor. Isotani et al. 15 found no significant differences between maternal education and the child's expressive language, but when correlating income with child's phrasal extension, a positive association was observed (p = 0.008), so that the higher the family income, the greater the child's phrasal extension.

Child development, especially that related to language, is greatly influenced by the social environment in which the child lives,30 and 31 and low income and educational level are variables that may be related to fewer opportunities for interactions between adult and child, collapse of the family structure, and school abandonment. Therefore, children born preterm whose families are in vulnerable situations may be more prone to developmental alterations and, thus, it is extremely important for health professionals to be aware of these issues.

Conclusion

When associating preterm birth with child language development, the literature demonstrated that prematurity is a risk for language development alterations, especially during early years. In the studies that compared preterm children and children born at term, there was evidence that preterm children had poorer performance in development indicators related to language. When considering the types of language, the expressive language was more impaired than the receptive.

Higher parental education and family income were identified as protective factors for language development, whereas lower BW and higher degree of prematurity were identified as risk factors. In the studies that analyzed different levels of prematurity and BW, it was observed that children born with lower GA and lower weight had worse performance in language assessment tests than children with higher weight and GA closer to 37 weeks.

As for the procedures used for language assessment, it was observed that a wide variety of tools were used in the studies, which makes it difficult to compare them. Thus, the authors emphasize the need for researchers to study the different tools in order to select the best for their objectives.

Hence, preterm infants and families in more vulnerable situations may be more prone to development alterations. Considering this issue is of utmost importance, health services have developed actions to follow these children regarding language development. Thus, it is essential that pediatricians be aware of the language development in these children so that adequate treatment can be provided.

Acknowledgments

To all graduate students and professors of the Postgraduate Program in Child and Adolescent Health of UNICAMP, who helped review this manuscript.

References

  • 1Brasil. Ministério da Saúde. Secretaria de Atenção à Saúde. Departamento de Ações Programáticas Estratégicas. Atenção à saúde do recém-nascido: guia para os profissionais de saúde. Ministério da Saúde, Secretaria de Atenção à Saúde, Departa- mento de Ações Programáticas Estratégicas. 2nd ed. Brasília: Ministério da Saúde, 2012. 4 v.:il- Série A. Normas e Manuais Técnicos.
  • 2Bühler KE, Limongi SC, Diniz EM. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009;67:242-9.
  • 3Da Silva ES, Nunes ML. The influence of gestational age and birth weight in the clinical assessment of the muscle tone of healthy term and preterm newborns. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2005;63:956-62.
  • 4Silveira MF, Santos I, Barros AJ, Matijasevich A, Barros FC, Victora CG. Increase in preterm births in Brazil: review of population-based studies. Rev Saude Publica. 2008;42:957-64.
  • 5Rugolo LM. Crescimento e desenvolvimento a longo prazo do prematuro extremo. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2005;8:S101-10.
  • 6McCormick MC. The contribution of low birth weight to infant mortality and childhood morbidity. N Engl J Med. 1985;312:82-90.
  • 7Vohr BR, Wright LL, Dusick AM, Mele L, Verter J, Steichen JJ, et al. Neurodevelopmental and functional outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants in the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Net- work, 1993-1994. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1216-26.
  • 8Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7.
  • 9Schirmer CR, Portuguez MW, Nunes ML. Clinical assessment of language development in children at age 3 years that were born preterm. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2006;64:926-31.
  • 10Doyle LW, Roberts G, Anderson PJ, Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. Outcomes at age 2 years of infants < 28 weeks' gestational age born in Victoria in 2005. J Pediatr. 2010;156:49-53.
  • 11Leone CR, Ramos JL. FAV O recém nascido pré-termo. In: Mar- condes E, Vaz FA, Ramos JL, Okay Y, editors. Pediatria básica. 9th ed. São Paulo: Savier; 2002.
  • 12Meio MD, Lopes CS, Morsch DS, Monteiro AP, Rocha SB, Borges RA, et al. Desenvolvimento cognitivo de crianças prematuras de muito baixo peso na idade pré-escolar. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2004;80:495-502.
  • 13Pereira MR, Funayama CA. Evaluation of some aspects of the acquisition and development of language in pre-term born chil- dren. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2004;62(3a):641-8.
  • 14Ishii C, Miranda CS, Isotani SM, Perissinoto J. Caracterização de comportamentos lingüísticos de crianças nascidas pre- maturas, aos quatro anos de idade. Rev CEFAC. 2006;8:147-54.
  • 15Isotani SM, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM, Perissinoto J. Expressive language of two year-old pre-term and full-term children. Pro Fono. 2009;21:155-9.
  • 16Lamonica DA, Picoloni MM. Habilidades do desenvolvimento de prematuros. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11:145-53.
  • 17Lamônica DA, Carlino FC, Alvarenga KdeF. Assessment of recep- tive and expressive auditory and visual functions in pre-term children. Pro Fono. 2010;22:19-24.
  • 18Lima MC, Ruivo NG, Casali RL, Françozo MdeC, Colella-Santos MF, Alves MC. Comparação do desenvolvimento da linguagem de crianças nascidas a termo e pré-termo com indicadores de risco para surdez. Distúrb comun. 2011;23:297-306.
  • 19Eickmann SH, Malkes NF, Lima MeC. Psychomotor development of preterm infants aged 6 to 12 months. Sao Paulo Med J. 2012;130:299-306.
  • 20Fernandes LV, Goulart AL, Santos AM, Barros MC, Guerra CC, Kopelman BI. Neurodevelopmental assessment of very low birth weight preterm infants at corrected age of 18-24 months by Bayley III scales. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2012;88:471-8.
  • 21Silveira KA, Enumo SR. Riscos biopsicossociais para o desen- volvimento de crianças prematuras e com baixo peso. Paidéia. 2012;22:335-45.
  • 22Maria-Mengel MR. Martins Linhares MB. Risk factors for infant developmental problems. Rev Lat Am Enfermagem. 2007;15:837-42.
  • 23Foster-Cohen S, Edgin JO, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J Child Lang. 2007;34:655-75.
  • 24PerissinotoJ, Isotani S.Desenvolvimento da linguagem: pro- grama de acompanhamento de recém nascidos de risco. In: Hernandez AM, editor. Conhecimentos essenciais para atender bem o neonato.Pulso: São José dos Campos (SP); 2003. p. 113-21.
  • 25Rutter M, Sroufe LA. Developmental psychopathology: concepts and challenges. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12:265-96.
  • 26Vieira ME, Linhares MB. Developmental outcomes and quality of life in children born preterm at preschool- and school-age. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2011;87:281-91.
  • 27Brocchi BS, Leme MI. A relação entre interação mãe-criança no desenvolvimento da linguagem oral de recém-nascidos pre- maturos. ACR. 2013;18:321-31.
  • 28Klein VC, Linhares MB. Prematuridade e interação mãe-criança: revisão sistemática da literatura. Psicol Estud. 2006;11:277-84.
  • 29Scopel RR, Souza VC, Lemos SM. A influência do ambi- ente familiar e escolar na aquisição e no desenvolvimento da linguagem: revisão de literatura. Rev CEFAC. 2012;14:732-41.
  • 30Vygotsky LS. Aprendizado e desenvolvimento. Um processo sócio-histórico. 4th ed. São Paulo: Scipione; 2003. 31. Vygotsky LS. Pensamento e linguagem. 4th ed. São Paulo: Martins Fontes; 2008.
  • Please cite this article as: Zerbeto AB, Cortelo FM, Filho ÉB. Association between gestational age and birth weight on the language development of Brazilian children: a systematic review. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2015;91:326-32.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Aug 2015

History

  • Received
    11 Nov 2014
  • Accepted
    27 Nov 2014
location_on
Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria Av. Carlos Gomes, 328 cj. 304, CEP: 90480-000 , Tel.: (+55 51) 3108-3328 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: assessoria@jped.com.br
rss_feed Acompanhe os números deste periódico no seu leitor de RSS
Acessibilidade / Reportar erro