Accessibility / Report Error

Social information as an entrainment cue for the circadian clock

Abstract

Animals adapt to the daily changes in their environmental conditions by means of genetically encoded circadian clocks. These clocks, found throughout the tree of life, regulate diverse biological functions, and allow periodical changes in physiology and behaviour. The molecular underpinnings of these clocks have been extensively studied across taxa, revealing a brain-based system that coordinates rhythmic activities through neuronal networks and signalling pathways. Entrainment, the alignment of internal rhythms with external cues or zeitgebers, is crucial for the adaptive value of these internal clocks. While the solar light-dark cycle is a primary zeitgeber for most animals, other relevant cues such as temperature, meal timing, predators, anxiety, fear, physical activity, and social interactions also play roles in entraining circadian clocks. The search of a detailed description of the circadian clocks is a goal for neurobiology and an area of growing societal interests. Moreover, as disruptions in circadian rhythms are implicated in various diseases, understanding the entrainment pathways contributes to developing interventions for improved wellbeing and health outcomes. This review focuses on socially relevant cues, examining their impact on animal physiology and behaviour, and explores the sensory pathways transmitting information to the central clock.

Keywords:
Circadian rhythms; entrainment; synchronization; social cues; sensory modalities

The circadian clock and its entrainment capabilities

Animals exhibit cyclic variations in behaviour and physiology, enabling them to anticipate and adapt to daily changes in their environment. These variations encompass well-known behavioural states like sleep-wake cycles as well as numerous subtle oscillations in neurological, metabolic, endocrine, cardiovascular, and immune functions. The underlying mechanism responsible for generating this behavioural and physiological rhythmicity is a genetically encoded molecular clock. These clocks, present across the tree of life, consist of interconnected components that regulate their own abundance and activity, resulting in rhythms that are close to 24 hours in duration and are hence referred to as circadian (from the Latin circa -about- dies -a day-) clocks (Kumar, 2017Kumar V (2017) Biological timekeeping: Clocks, rhythms and behaviour. Springer, New Delhi.).

Understanding the physiological processes regulating the central circadian clock is of utmost importance. Disruptions in circadian rhythms are associated with numerous diseases that significantly influence both health and the economy. These diseases include sleep disorders, type II diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, autoimmune diseases, depression, bipolar disorders, and many others (Meyer et al., 2022Meyer N, Harvey AG, Lockley SW and Dijk DJ (2022) Circadian rhythms and disorders of the timing of sleep. Lancet 400:1061-1078.; Rijo-Ferreira and Takahashi, 2019Rijo-Ferreira F and Takahashi JS (2019) Genomics of circadian rhythms in health and disease. Genome Med 11:82.). Therefore, understanding the relationships between the external factors that influence the central clock and their effect on the circadian physiology holds great relevance. In the long term, this will help to elucidate the underlying mechanisms of multiple diseases and will contribute to develop effective interventions to mitigate their impact.

Extensive research has shed light on the molecular components of the circadian clock across taxa, offering insights into the regulatory machinery behind the generation of the close-to-24-hour rhythms. In animals, the molecular machinery of the circadian clock is primarily located in the brain where specialized cells known as clock cells communicate with each other to coordinate their activities and regulate downstream circadian rhythms. The communication within the master pacemaker cells involves intricate networks of neuronal connections and signalling pathways that have different properties depending on the taxa of focus, and have been extensively studied (Cox and Takahashi, 2019Cox KH and Takahashi JS (2019) Circadian clock genes and the transcriptional architecture of the clock mechanism. J Mol Endocrinol 63:R93-R102.; Shafer et al., 2022Shafer OT, Gutierrez GJ, Li K, Mildenhall A, Spira D, Marty J, Lazar AA and Fernandez MP (2022) Connectomic analysis of the Drosophila lateral neuron clock cells reveals the synaptic basis of functional pacemaker classes. Elife 11:e79139.; Reinhard et al., 2023Reinhard N, Fukuda A, Manoli G, Derksen E, Saito A, Möller G, Sekiguchi M, Rieger D, Helfrich-Förster C, Yoshii T et al. (2023) Synaptic and peptidergic connectomes of the Drosophila circadian clock. bioRxiv: 2023.09.11.557222.; Starnes and Jones, 2023Starnes AN and Jones JR (2023) Inputs and outputs of the mammalian circadian clock. Biology 12:508.). At the molecular level, the cogs of the clock employ a machinery that involves multiple layers of control, ranging from the regulation of epigenomic marks to the oscillation of posttranslational modifications (Beckwith and Yanovsky, 2014Beckwith EJ and Yanovsky MJ (2014) Circadian regulation of gene expression: At the crossroads of transcriptional and post-transcriptional regulatory networks. Curr Opin Genet Dev 27:35-42.). A key common aspect of all circadian systems is that the central neuronal pacemaker synchronizes the rest of the body, influencing multiple tissues and other secondary cell-autonomous oscillators, aiming to align internal rhythms. To achieve a meaningful synchronization, the master circadian clock relies on external stimuli known as zeitgebers (German: “time givers”). Thus, entrainment is the alignment of the internal biological clock rhythm to external time cues, and key aspects of entrainment are the resulting phase and periodicity of the controlled variables. In lay terms, entrainment is the way that our internal clocks are reset to reflect the natural periods of day and night that occur in our environment. While the daily solar light-dark cycle serves as the primary zeitgeber for many animals, the relevance of the other external cues is determined by the physiology and ecology of each species, and it is shaped through evolution. Thus, animals also respond to other cues such as temperature, meal timing, presence of predators, elements that produce anxiety or fear, their own physical activity, and social interactions to entrain their circadian clock (Kumar, 2017Kumar V (2017) Biological timekeeping: Clocks, rhythms and behaviour. Springer, New Delhi.).

The focus of this review will be on the relevant cues generated by social interactions that can influence the phase of physiology and behaviour in animals, and what we know about the sensory pathways that convey the information from the periphery to the master clock. Since the knowledge on circadian clock and their entrainment pathways is vast, we will refer the reader to wonderful reviews and textbooks conducting an in-depth study of how other relevant cues entrain the circadian clock (Bloch et al., 2013Bloch G, Herzog ED, Levine JD and Schwartz WJ (2013) Socially synchronized circadian oscillators. Proc Biol Sci 280:20130035.; Kumar, 2017Kumar V (2017) Biological timekeeping: Clocks, rhythms and behaviour. Springer, New Delhi.; Foster et al., 2020Foster RG, Hughes S and Peirson SN (2020) Circadian photoentrainment in mice and humans. Biology (Basel) 9:180.).

Social interactions are key factor influencing fitness, are they relevant for the circadian clock?

For all living organisms, the presence or absence of conspecifics and other members of their ecosystem are critical for survival. Therefore, synchronizing their activities in relation to the potential for interactions, whether inter- or trans-specific, is equally crucial. There is abundant evidence of synchronization phenomena, whether to coexist in time and space or to avoid such interactions. However, distinguishing between synchronization of events and an entrainment of the circadian system that establishes a long lasting phase over time, allowing anticipation of an up-coming change in conditions, is not an easy task regardless of the nature of the synchronizing stimulus. Figure 1 shows two different theoretical examples where an animal that is free running with its own circadian time (CT), established by the endogenous period and phase of its internal clock, is subjected to a training event. During this event, the animal shows synchronization to the oscillation in the stimulus, and it runs with the zeitgeber time (ZT). Only when the presence of the external stimulus ends one can evaluate if the zeitgeber was able to impact the master clock and set a new phase (Figure 1, left) or the animals returns to its original phase (Figure 1, right). Thus, evidence that synchronization is based on a true entrainment of the circadian clock, i.e., a modification of the phase of these internal rhythms, requires an experimental approach that it is not always present or accessible.

Figure 1 -
True-entrainment versus synchronization of activities without a proper entrainment of the circadian clock. The two hypothetical actograms describe two fundamentally different situations. In both situations, a generic animal starts free-running without any environmental cues (grey background) and shows a slightly short period with a certain phase (red line). Then, each animal is confronted with an oscillating external stimulus (green or purple). In both cases, these stimuli are able to synchronize the activity to a 24h cycle. The key difference is that the green stimulus is of such nature that it manages to entrain the clock, and after the removal of the stimulus, the activity follows from the new phase (green line), free-running with the same periodicity but in a different phase. In the case of the purple stimulus, the behaviour goes back to the original red phase, ignoring the experience of the purple stimulus. The plots represent fabricated double-plotted actograms, where the y-axis indicates the days of the experiment, each row represents two consecutive days, and the black bars indicate the magnitude of a certain behaviour or physiological property that is oscillating in a circadian fashion with one peak every circa 24h. The x-axis represents time in hours, when external cues are present the time is commonly referred to as Zeitgeber Time (ZT), and the standard is to indicate ZT0 as the initiation of the stimulus (typically light ON, the beginning of the day). When the animals is free running (no external cues), time is referred as Circadian Time (CT).

From an experimental perspective, it requires precise and challenging interventions that are often very difficult to perform. The primary challenge lays on the direct influence of social inputs on behaviour, which can alter the expression of daily cycles without necessarily impacting the intrinsic circadian clock or can activate temporal conditioning mechanisms. When a stimulus induces behaviours at a time away from their typical circadian phase, it is described as “masking” the genuine circadian phase. This veiling effect makes it laborious to discern whether observed behavioural changes are a result of circadian clock entrainment or merely a response to immediate social cues. Researchers face the complexity of distinguishing between genuine circadian modulation and behavioural responses induced by external stimuli, requiring meticulous and original experimental designs to tease apart these entangled interactions (Aschoff, 1960Aschoff J (1960) Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 25:11-28.; Mrosovsky, 1999Mrosovsky N (1999) Masking: History, definitions, and measurement. Chronobiol Int 16:415-429.).

The circadian timekeeping system in humans, as in other mammals, is governed by environmental cues such as light quality and intensity, and temperature (Dibner et al., 2010Dibner C, Schibler U and Albrecht U (2010) The mammalian circadian timing system: Organization and coordination of central and peripheral clocks. Annu Rev Physiol 72:517-549.). This system is synchronized to geophysical time predominantly through photic cues detected by the retina. Short exposures to low light intensity efficiently phase-shift the circadian clock, suppresses melatonin production, and induces alertness (Chang et al., 2012Chang AM, Santhi N, St Hilaire M, Gronfier C, Bradstreet DS, Duffy JF, Lockley SW, Kronauer RE and Czeisler CA (2012) Human responses to bright light of different durations. J Physiol 590:3103-3112.). In line with that, individuals with total blindness often exhibit circadian rhythm abnormalities, with a significant proportion of subjects experiencing free-running rhythms and some struggling with recurrent insomnia and daytime sleepiness (Sack et al., 1992Sack RL, Lewy AJ, Blood ML, Keith LD and Nakagawa H (1992) Circadian rhythm abnormalities in totally blind people: incidence and clinical significance. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 75:127-134.). This underscores the pivotal role of the light-dark cycle in synchronizing the human circadian system. However, groundbreaking experiments done by one of the founding fathers of modern chronobiology, Jürgen Aschoff, in 1971Aschoff J, Fatranska M, Giedke H, Doerr P, Stamm D and Wisser H (1971) Human circadian rhythms in continuous darkness: Entrainment by social cues. Science 171:213-215. highlighted the profound influence of social cues as primary entrainment factors in humans (Aschoff et al., 1971Aschoff J, Fatranska M, Giedke H, Doerr P, Stamm D and Wisser H (1971) Human circadian rhythms in continuous darkness: Entrainment by social cues. Science 171:213-215.). Carrying out experiments in total darkness, he and his team showed that social cues are key to entrain human circadian rhythms. They showed that in the absence of light but within a strong social context, the expected drift in physiological or psychological functions was not observed, underscoring the potency of social cues in orchestrating circadian regulation. Then, the growing idea that emerges is that maintaining alignment between photic and non-photic cues is paramount for the proper functioning of the circadian clock. Disruptions can manifest in various forms, including a mismatch between an individual’s self-selected cycles of light and dark and the natural day-night cycle. Several socially imposed factors, such as shift work, daylight saving time, and geographic location within a time zone, as well as travelling across longitudes, have been explored as potential sources of circadian disruption. These factors serve as particularly relevant situations for studying the consequences of circadian misalignment on health, shedding light on the intricate relationship between environmental cues, circadian rhythms, and their implications for human well-being.

Beyond the reported effect of social entrainment in humans, many extraordinary examples came from a variety of species in which we have different degrees of knowledge about the role of socialization on the clock and the mechanistic underpinnings (Figure 2). Experiments on bats (Hipposideros speoris) that inhabit caves inherently provide a context of socialization in constant darkness, serving as a valuable biological model for investigating non-photic entrainment of circadian rhythms (Marimuthu et al., 1978Marimuthu G, Subbaraj R and Chandrashekaran MK (1978) Social synchronization of the activity rhythm in a cave-dwelling insectivorous bat. Naturwissenschaften 65:600.). Seminal work revealed that caged bats, when surrounded by free-flying conspecifics, exhibit synchronization of activity with colony-wide patterns (Marimuthu et al., 1981Marimuthu G, Rajan S and Chandrashekaran MK (1981) Social entrainment of the circadian rhythm in the flight activity of the microchiropteran bat Hipposideros speoris. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:147-150.). This observation points to the ability of bats to communicate and socially synchronize their circadian rhythms. Furthermore, the study found that isolated bats placed in a cave of a different species free run in their circadian activity, indicating a species-specific element in the synchronization process. It is worth mentioning that this initial line of experiments did not explore the mechanisms of entrainment, the potential of these cues to induce phase shifts or the effect they have on the molecular clock. However, they do unravel the social dynamics of bats in their natural habitats and contribute to our understanding of species-specific influences on circadian regulation in the absence of external environmental light. Another similar example of social synchronization in nature comes from a study on the Canadian beaver (Castor canadensis) during winter, period in which they remain in their lodges covered of snow and ice, deprived of light inputs (Bovet and Oertli, 1974Bovet J and Oertli EF (1974) Free-running circadian activity rhythms in free-living beaver (Castor canadensis). J Comp Physiol 92:1-10.). A compelling series of recordings showed that a family of animals free-run with a single coherent 27-hour rhythm, rather than isolated individual rhythms, supporting the idea that phase synchrony was sustained by social interactions.

Figure 2 -
Sensory modalities of the social cues that entrain the circadian clock across species. A schematic representation of the taxonomic relationship (based on the NCBI Taxonomy) between animals mentioned along the review. The different colours represent the diverse range of modalities that have been tested to have an influence on the circadian clock.

There are also well documented examples of the lack of effect of social interaction on circadian activity rhythms in certain conditions. For instance, experiments with the nocturnal sugar glider (Petaurus breviceps), a small omnivorous Australian possum, showed that members of a pair maintained under constant illumination are not mutually synchronized and free-run with different circadian period lengths. While this particular set of experiments did not show social synchronization, it does not exclude the possibility of this phenomenon in this species. A similar experiment but studying deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus) showed proper social synchronization in which the activity rhythm of the dominant mouse entrains the activity rhythm of the subordinate (Crowley and Bovet, 1980Crowley M and Bovet J (1980) Social synchronization of circadian rhythms in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:99-105.). On the same line, solid evidence showed entrainment and phase shifting to brief agonistic encounters between male golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) (Mrosovsky, 1988Mrosovsky N (1988) Phase response curves for social entrainment. J Comp Physiol A 162:35-46.). These experiments showed that repeated social interactions at the same time of day could entrain activity rhythms in a way consistent with the shape of the phase response curves, placing social cues on an equal footing with light as a zeitgeber. As well as same-sex interaction, opposite-sex interactions are key for fitness. Work with mice (Mus musculus) has shown that both male and female free-running subjects exposed to the opposite sex exhibited a phase advance at circadian time 3 (CT3), delay at CT21, and no response at CT9, while only males responded with a phase advance at CT15 (Sonker and Singaravel, 2021Sonker P, Cherukalady R and Singaravel M (2021) Effect of periodic social interaction and odour presentation of same and opposite-sex conspecifics on free-running mice. Chronobiol Int 38:1714-1725.). Beyond the clear effect of social activity on the circadian clock, these experiments also point out to a gender difference in circadian clock responses for social cues, drawing a more complex and interesting landscape. Finally, probably the most elementary social interaction is the bond between a caring progenitor and its offspring. Evidence from different mammals demonstrated that maternal contact is crucial for coordinating the circadian clock during late fetal and early neonatal life, and this interaction is a well-documented example of effective social entrainment of the circadian clock (Sumova et al., 2006Sumova A, Bendova Z, Sladek M, El-Hennamy R, Laurinova K, Jindrakova Z and Illnerova H (2006) Setting the biological time in central and peripheral clocks during ontogenesis. FEBS Lett 580:2836-2842.). In rats (Rattus rattus), it has been shown that maternal care improves offspring’s circadian system and health (Olejnikova et al., 2018Olejnikova L, Polidarova L, Behuliak M, Sladek M and Sumova A (2018) Circadian alignment in a foster mother improves the offspring’s pathological phenotype. J Physiol 596:5757-5775.). Moreover, the circadian rest-activity cycles of pups in the night-active mouse Mus booduga are regulated by cyclic presence and absence of their mother, playing a role as zeitgeber (Viswanathan and Chandrashekaran, 1985Viswanathan N and Chandrashekaran MK (1985) Cycles of presence and absence of mother mouse entrain the circadian clock of pups. Nature 317:530-531.). This is also the case for new-born rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) pups, which suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN), the master pacemaker in the mammalian brain, show endogenous 24‐h rhythmicity in the expression of the clock genes Per1, Per2, and Bmal1, and it is reported that these rhythms are entrained by a one-a-day nursing event (Caldelas et al., 2009Caldelas I, Gonzalez B, Montufar-Chaveznava R and Hudson R (2009) Endogenous clock gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of previsual newborn rabbits is entrained by nursing. Dev Neurobiol 69:47-59.).

The mechanisms behind social entrainment

As mentioned, while there is ample evidence of synchronization by social cues and good evidence for true entrainment of the molecular clock, understanding the underlying mechanisms of circadian entrainment in response to social cues poses challenges in experimental design and execution. Mechanistic data supports the idea that light is the main entrainment cue for most animals, meanwhile, social zeitgebers could reach circadian clocks through different pathways. For instance, social cues could represent and induce a variety of internal states and employ pathways encoding stress, arousal, and fear or directly elicit locomotion. Alternatively, social information can reach the master clock via sensory-specific input pathways that transduce visual, tactile, auditory, gustatory and/or olfactory information.

Behavioural arousal in mammals can induce non-photic circadian clock resetting and modulate its response to light-dark cycles, potentially influencing human circadian rhythms (Webb et al., 2014Webb IC, Antle MC and Mistlberger RE (2014) Regulation of circadian rhythms in mammals by behavioral arousal. Behav Neurosci 128:304-325.) (Mistlberger and Antle, 2011Mistlberger RE and Antle MC (2011) Entrainment of circadian clocks in mammals by arousal and food. Essays Biochem 49:119-136.). This non-photic entrainment has both biological and therapeutic implications, and finds applications in experimental design for behavioural neuroscience studies. Importantly, arousal-induced phase shifts in the circadian clock are mediated by the cholinergic forebrain arousal system that reaches the SCN (Yamakawa et al., 2016Yamakawa GR, Basu P, Cortese F, MacDonnell J, Whalley D, Smith VM and Antle MC (2016) The cholinergic forebrain arousal system acts directly on the circadian pacemaker. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:13498-13503). This is evidenced by atropine (a potent competitive and reversible antagonist of the muscarinic acetylcholine receptors) injections into the SCN that block arousal-induced phase shifts. These observations highlight the pivotal role of cholinergic pathways in the complex interplay between behavioural arousal and circadian rhythms. In addition, the thalamic intergeniculate leaflet plays a described role in non-photic entrainment of the circadian clock of Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus), with its loss affecting non-photic entrainment and activity onset (Maywood et al., 1997Maywood ES, Smith E, Hall SJ and Hastings MH (1997) A thalamic contribution to arousal-induced, non-photic entrainment of the circadian clock of the Syrian hamster. Eur J Neurosci 9:1739-1747.).

The mechanisms behind social entrainment: the case for sound social cues

Avian circadian rhythms, akin to those observed in mammals, regulate a spectrum of vital biological processes such as sleep-wake cycles, visual function, song production, migratory patterns, and orientation. The parallels between avian and mammalian circadian systems position birds as valuable models for understanding the dynamics of human biological clocks. Studies involving house sparrows (Passer domesticus) have presented compelling evidence that sound cues possess the capacity to shape circadian rhythms (Menaker and Eskin, 1966Menaker M and Eskin A (1966) Entrainment of circadian rhythms by sound in Passer domesticus. Science 154:1579-1581.), representing a pivotal stride in our comprehension of the entrainment process. These findings mark the inaugural demonstration of a direct correlation between sound stimuli and the modulation of a biological clock, reminiscent of the role of vibrations in honeybees (Beer et al., 2016Beer K, Steffan-Dewenter I, Härtel S and Helfrich-Förster C (2016) A new device for monitoring individual activity rhythms of honey bees reveals critical effects of the social environment on behavior. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 202:555-565.) (see section below). Moreover, one notable experiment elucidated that the introduction of a musically enriched bird song stimulus during the early subjective day phase can induce a significant advancement in circadian rhythms in humans (Goel, 2006Goel N (2006) An arousing, musically enhanced bird song stimulus mediates circadian rhythm phase advances in dim light. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 291:R822-R827.). This advancement is postulated to stem from an arousal-mediated effect previously described, underscoring the multifaceted nature of the mechanisms involved. Intriguingly, the non-photic stimulus not only influences the timing of the circadian rhythm but can also dictate whether the resynchronization leans towards a phase advance or delay.

The intricate interplay between auditory cues and the temporal alignment of biological clocks, as well as the underlying mechanisms governing it, awaits further elucidation. Importantly, investigations with animals more amenable to genetic manipulations may prove instrumental in unravelling the intricacies of these mechanisms, as well as other perceptual modalities. A key study employing the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster shed light on the role of diurnal variations in mechanical vibrations as potential resetters of circadian clock phases (Simoni et al., 2014Simoni A, Wolfgang W, Topping MP, Kavlie RG, Stanewsky R and Albert JT (2014) A mechanosensory pathway to the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 343:525-528.). Although the source of this mechanical vibration in nature can be of many sorts, and not necessarily represent socially relevant information, the mechanistic insight allows further evaluation. The chordotonal organs, situated in limb joints of insects, provide the animals with neural impulses for posture and position detection as well as sensitivity to mechanical stimuli, specifically vibrations and movements in their environment, reminiscent of the mammalian ear. This organ plays a role in coordinating how temperature affects circadian cycles in flies (Sehadova et al., 2009Sehadova H, Glaser FT, Gentile C, Simoni A, Giesecke A, Albert JT and Stanewsky R (2009) Temperature entrainment of Drosophila’s circadian clock involves the gene nocte and signaling from peripheral sensory tissues to the brain. Neuron 64:251-266.) and has the potential of acting as mediator for the feedback of various behaviours onto the endogenous biological clock. In this regard, the processing of mechanosensory inputs to entrain the circadian clock of Drosophila melanogaster is well described (Simoni et al., 2014Simoni A, Wolfgang W, Topping MP, Kavlie RG, Stanewsky R and Albert JT (2014) A mechanosensory pathway to the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 343:525-528.). Specifically, the daily locomotor activity of wild-type flies can be synchronized using cycles of 12 hours of vibration and 12 hours of quiet. Behavioural synchronization to sound cycles requires a functional clock and chordotonal organs, and are accompanied by phase alterations in the daily oscillations in the abundance of the central clock protein PERIOD in the brain pacemaker. This highlights the crucial role of proprioceptive organs in providing feedback for maintaining the alignment of circadian rhythm in correspondence to stimulus-induced activity. As with sound-induced entrainment in birds and humans, the molecular intricacies of how mechanical vibrations integrate into the circadian system remain an intriguing area for future exploration, where studies with genetically amenable organisms may prove fundamental.

The mechanisms behind social entrainment: the case for olfactorysocial cues

Odorants have a key evolutionary significance for various fitness-related behaviours such as mate choice, predator avoidance, and foraging strategies. Many studies suggest that social olfactory cues contribute to circadian entrainment and the resetting of the clock in various species, influencing physiological, behavioural rhythms, and memory recall.

Odorants can act as a circadian time cue, influencing circadian locomotor behaviour in mammals (Figure 3), with effects even more pronounced in the absence of the SCN, but require the presence of a functional canonical molecular clock (Abraham et al., 2013Abraham U, Saleh M and Kramer A (2013) Odor is a time cue for circadian behavior. J Biol Rhythms 28:26-37.). In term of mechanisms, the entrainment of rabbit pups to maternal nursing described earlier provides a well-established example (Caldelas et al., 2009Caldelas I, Gonzalez B, Montufar-Chaveznava R and Hudson R (2009) Endogenous clock gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of previsual newborn rabbits is entrained by nursing. Dev Neurobiol 69:47-59.). Maternal olfactory cues in the milk, particularly the mammary pheromone 2MB2, effectively synchronize the circadian system of new-born rabbits, affecting core body temperature, locomotor activity, and metabolic variables (Caldelas et al., 2009Caldelas I, Gonzalez B, Montufar-Chaveznava R and Hudson R (2009) Endogenous clock gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of previsual newborn rabbits is entrained by nursing. Dev Neurobiol 69:47-59.; Montufar-Chaveznava et al., 2013Montufar-Chaveznava R, Trejo-Munoz L, Hernandez-Campos O, Navarrete E and Caldelas I (2013) Maternal olfactory cues synchronize the circadian system of artificially raised newborn rabbits. PLoS One 8:e74048.). At the same time, the relevance of odorants is not always sufficient for entrainment. One study in mice reported proper entrainment upon social interaction, while exclusive odorant stimulation was not enough to entrain the clock and only produced masking of the overt activity rhythms (Sonker et al., 2021Sonker P, Cherukalady R and Singaravel M (2021) Effect of periodic social interaction and odour presentation of same and opposite-sex conspecifics on free-running mice. Chronobiol Int 38:1714-1725.). Importantly, in a different set of pairing experiment, it has been shown that social interactions with conspecifics of the opposite-sex up-regulated c-fos/C-FOS in the olfactory bulb, per-1/PER-1 in the SCN, C-FOS, and PER-1 in the piriform cortex (a brain region involved in the processing and perception of olfactory information) of both male and female when the interaction between a free-running individual and an entrained one is presented at CT3; pointing to a clear involvement of key genes in olfactory and circadian dedicated areas (Sonker and Singaravel, 2021Sonker P and Singaravel M (2021) Gender difference in circadian clock responses for social interaction with conspecific of the opposite-sex. Chronobiol Int 38:212-223.).

Figure 3 -
Olfactory cues are key to social entrainment in both mammals and insects. A. For the genetic model system Drosophila melanogaster, there is a very precise description of the detection and processing of chemosensory cues, as well as a detailed mapping of the clock neurons and their connectivity. In particular, olfactory molecules bind to olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) located at the antennae and maxillary palps. These cells send their projections to the antennal lobe (AL) where they form glomeruli and make synapses with projection neurons (PNs), the output neurons of the AL. These cells send projections to the dorsal protocerebrum, transmitting the olfactory information to multimodal processing structures, such as mushroom bodies (MBs) and lateral horn (LH). These projection neurons are the main candidates to drive relevant social information to the clock cells, either by direct synaptic contact or by means of intermediate neuronal circuits that con involve neuromodulators or neuropeptides, both common tools of the circadian system. Thus, we still do not know how the olfactory system and the circadian network are connected (dashed arrows marked with a question mark in the scheme). B. In mammals, we also have a precise description of both, the olfactory system and the circadian pacemaker, the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN). In particular, the vertebrate olfactory system is analogous to the insect system: the olfactory receptor neurons are located on the olfactory epithelium and send their axons to the olfactory bulb (OB), where they make synapses with mitral cells that project to other areas of the brain, such as the piriform cortex (PC). It is postulated that this area is critical to send social olfactory cues relevant to the SCN, but the connection between the olfactory system and the circadian network is still not described.

A highly social and diurnal rodent indigenous to central Chile offered fundamental insights into the entrainment of circadian clocks by social olfactory cues. The common degus (Octodon degus) is known for its communal burrows and cooperative behaviours such as communal nesting and nursing each other’s young, providing an ecological context where social cues may be particularly relevant. A series of works suggests that the circadian clock of this rodent is not solely influenced by external stimuli like light but also responds to social information (Hagenauer and Lee, 2008Hagenauer MH and Lee TM (2008) Circadian organization of the diurnal Caviomorph rodent, Octodon degus. Biol Rhythm Res 39:269-289.). Importantly, the observed entrainment-dependent mechanisms and variations in chronotype parallel those found in humans. In particular, pairing experiments conducted with animals in a free-running condition unveiled that social information, in the absence of light, holds the capacity for partial entrainment. Such social cues induce changes in period and phase of free-running rhythms. Interestingly, sex-specific differences were noted in this entrainment capacity (Goel and Lee, 1996Goel N and Lee TM (1996) Relationship of circadian activity and social behaviors to reentrainment rates in diurnal Octodon degus (Rodentia). Physiol Behav 59:817-826.). Free-running females housed with entrained females exhibited faster entrainment compared to those housed with entrained males. Moreover, in this context, an increased frequency of scent-marking behaviours was observed in comparison to same-sex housing conditions. Crucially, the same group of researchers underscored the relevance of the olfactory system in the entrainment process (Governale and Lee, 2001Governale MM and Lee TM (2001) Olfactory cues accelerate reentrainment following phase shifts and entrain free-running rhythms in female Octodon degus (Rodentia). J Biol Rhythms 16:489-501.). Olfactory bulbectomy experiments conclusively demonstrated that the olfactory bulbs and chemosensory cues play a necessary role in socially facilitated entrainment (Goel and Lee, 1997Goel N and Lee TM (1997) Olfactory bulbectomy impedes social but not photic reentrainment of circadian rhythms in female Octodon degus. J Biol Rhythms 12:362-370.). This finding provides robust evidence that chemosensory stimuli can modulate the circadian system in a diurnal rodent, shedding light on the intricate interplay between social cues, olfaction, and circadian regulation; however, the mechanistic connection between the olfactory and circadian systems is not described. In agreement with this line of research, experiments employing isolated honeybees also point towards the importance of olfactory cues involved in circadian entrainment within the colony (see section below).

In this regard, key mechanistic insight comes from the social circadian entrainment in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster (Figure 3). Experiments show that group-housed flies compared to isolated flies exhibit better phase coherence, meaning that the circadian clocks of the flies housed together are more synchronous than those of isolated flies (Levine et al., 2002Levine JD, Funes P, Dowse HB and Hall JC (2002) Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 298:2010-2012.). Furthermore, social interactions between genetically arrhythmic and wild type flies generate a more dispersed peak of locomotor activity, consequently, worsening the phase coherence in the rhythmic group. This shows that the genetically encoded circadian clock drives the expression of synchronizing olfactory cues that influence the overt circadian rhythm of surrounding conspecific. Moreover, the size of the group of flies is important on the effect of the social context in the circadian clock since larger groups induce a more prominent effect and pairwise social interactions do not show alterations (Lone and Sharma, 2011Lone SR and Sharma VK (2011) Social synchronization of circadian locomotor activity rhythm in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol 214:3742-3750. ). Critically, it has been shown that odorants may be responsible for the social synchronization of the circadian rhythm described before. Wild type flies who received “fly air” (a group of individuals who received pumped air from a vial with food and flies) show a stronger synchronization than flies who received “neutral air” (air from a vial without flies) (Levine et al., 2002Levine JD, Funes P, Dowse HB and Hall JC (2002) Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 298:2010-2012.). This means that individuals who only received chemosensory signals from another group of flies synchronize their phase of circadian activity. Continuing with this line of experiments, social interactions between olfactory-impaired mutants and genetically arrhythmic flies did not alter the circadian rhythm of the first group, different from what was shown before with the effect of the arrhythmic flies on the wild type ones (Levine et al., 2002Levine JD, Funes P, Dowse HB and Hall JC (2002) Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 298:2010-2012.). This result supports the idea that without a functional olfactory sensory system, the circadian clock of flies cannot be entrained by a social context under constant darkness. However, the olfactory impaired mutant employed is a pleiotropic mutation in a voltage-gated sodium channel, and a more specific olfactory mutant needs to be tested to fully support the conclusion.

In order to dissect the chemosensory signalling that entrains the circadian clock of flies under social interactions, Krupp et al. (2008Krupp JJ, Kent C, Billeter JC, Azanchi R, So AK, Schonfeld JA, Smith BP, Lucas C and Levine JD (2008) Social experience modifies pheromone expression and mating behavior in male Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol 18:1373-1383.) performed various experiments analysing the pheromone expression only in male flies and how this was correlated with the circadian rhythm at the molecular level. They demonstrated that three key clock genes (period, timeless and Clock) are expressed in oenocytes, the epidermal cells responsible for the production of cuticular hydrocarbons in insects, implying that these cells could act as peripheral clock. Some of these hydrocarbons can act as pheromones, and their accumulation on the body surface is per-dependent. Social interactions between heterogeneous groups (wild-type and arrhythmic male flies) and homogeneous groups (genotypically identical wild-type individuals) showed different patterns of clock gene expression in oenocytes. Furthermore, these two treatments differentially affect the overall levels of several cuticular pheromones and their temporal patterns. This evidence supports the hypothesis that pheromones are responsible for the synchronization of the phase of circadian activity in male flies.

Although the nature of the signal and the system that detects it seems clear, the neuronal mechanisms that convey the signals to the circadian clock have not been elucidated. A key piece of evidence shows that electroantennogram responses to an appetitive olfactory stimulus show circadian variability in their amplitude (Krishnan et al., 1999Krishnan B, Dryer SE and Hardin PE (1999) Circadian rhythms in olfactory responses of Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 400:375-378.). A molecular clock that depends on the genes period and timeless controls this rhythm, but it is independent of the pacemaker neurons that mediate locomotor activity rhythms. This finding supports the hypothesis of the presence of peripheral oscillators in the antennae that mediate a circadian sensitivity to odorants (Krishnan et al., 1999Krishnan B, Dryer SE and Hardin PE (1999) Circadian rhythms in olfactory responses of Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 400:375-378.; Tanoue et al., 2004Tanoue S, Krishnan P, Krishnan B, Dryer SE and Hardin PE (2004) Circadian clocks in antennal neurons are necessary and sufficient for olfaction rhythms in Drosophila. Curr Biol 14:638-649.; Krishnan et al., 2008Krishnan P, Chatterjee A, Tanoue S and Hardin PE (2008) Spike amplitude of single-unit responses in antennal sensillae is controlled by the Drosophila circadian clock. Curr Biol 18:803-807.).

Altogether, these experiments propose the interesting hypothesis that circadian entrainment by social cues may rely on both the cyclic production of odorants and an oscillation in olfactory perception, demonstrating the complexity of the process and the degree of evolutionary pressure to remain in synchrony with the environment.

Time synchronization in eusocial insects

Regarding the role of social cues to organize physiology and behaviour, true social, or eusocial, animals are probably the epitome in the animal kingdom, and the role of these cues on the circadian clock has been studied too. Eusociality refers to an advanced level of social organization in which individuals within a group display reproductive division of labour, cooperative care of offspring and overlapping generations.

In particular, ants represent an extremely diverse group of animals showing eusocial organizations with highly developed social communication skills that modulate their behaviour. The knowledge of the molecular clock in ants has grown exponentially, however our understanding of it is limited when compared with other model systems, such as flies or bees. Specifically, as other animals, ants also show 24 hours oscillations on the expression pattern of the central clock gene period (Ingram et al., 2009Ingram KK, Krummey S and LeRoux M (2009) Expression patterns of a circadian clock gene are associated with age-related polyethism in harvester ants, Pogonomyrmex occidentalis. BMC Ecol 9:7.; Das and de Bekker, 2022Das B and de Bekker C (2022) Time-course RNASeq of Camponotus floridanus forager and nurse ant brains indicate links between plasticity in the biological clock and behavioral division of labor. BMC Genomics 23:57.). An interesting study by Fujioka et al. (2017Fujioka H, Abe MS, Fuchikawa T, Tsuji K, Shimada M and Okada Y (2017) Ant circadian activity associated with brood care type. Biol Lett 13:20160743.) on the role of social cues on the circadian rhythms of nurse ants (Diacamma sp.) showed that isolated nurses show circadian rhythms in locomotion. However, nurses that were kept with conspecifics in the first stages of development, such as eggs and larvae (that require high levels of care by the nurse) show weaker oscillation, with around the clock activity levels, suggesting that the task performed by the animals (or cues associated to the task) are relevant information that can modulate the overt locomotor rhythms.

To study if the role of an ant worker in a group can influence the temporal pattern of activity, Fujioka et al. (2019Fujioka H, Abe MS and Okada Y (2019) Ant activity-rest rhythms vary with age and interaction frequencies of workers. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 73:30.) tracked the rest-activity profile of individual ants that were housed in groups that included animals with different roles (i.e. young nurses and old foragers) and found that these interactions induced the loss of the rhythmic activity. In isolation, both young and old worker ants showed circadian activity rhythms that were abolished when these workers were grouped with other workers from different age groups (Fujioka et al., 2019Fujioka H, Abe MS and Okada Y (2019) Ant activity-rest rhythms vary with age and interaction frequencies of workers. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 73:30.).

In a follow up study, the authors tracked large groups of Diacamma sp. (between 88 to 194 animals) and also found that workers of different ages (young and old) did not show circadian oscillation in their locomotion when grouped (Fujioka et al., 2021Fujioka H, Abe MS and Okada Y (2021) Individual ants do not show activity-rest rhythms in nest conditions. J Biol Rhythms 36:297-310.). These results suggest that the socialization provided by the colony has a clear effect on the locomotor pattern of activity of individual ants. Nevertheless, the nature and identity of the social cues underlying this influence over the locomotion rhythms have not been identified yet. In conclusion, these data show that the breeding needs of the colony can impose an effect on the activity of individual nurses and control their behaviour regardless of the entrained circadian rhythms. What remains to be addressed is the effect of nursing behaviours on the endogenous circadian rhythms of individual ants, distinguishing between effects on the clock entrainment or a masking effect on the locomotion levels. At the same, in future experiments would be critical for the understanding of the ant’s circadian system to focus on a different output, considering that the locomotor activity may be detached from a circadian control, but many aspects of the animal physiology and behaviour may remain running under the control of the circadian clock, and demonstrate daily oscillations.

Another fascinating example of true social organization are honeybees (Apis mellifera). They exhibit a highly organized colony structure where the cooperative efforts of the different castes contribute to the overall success of the colony. Within a hive, there is a single reproductive female, the queen, with the primary role of laying eggs. The rest of the female animals, the worker bees, assume various tasks such as foraging, nursing and defending the colony depending on their age. Bees exhibit very developed communication skills that includes pheromones, food exchange, temperature signals and even complex behaviours such as dances, further exemplifying the importance of collaboration for the thriving of the colony (Seeley, 1995Seeley TD (1995) The wisdom of the hive: The social physiology of honey bee colonies. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.). Are social cues capable of entraining honeybees’ circadian clock? In an environment as complex as a hive it is very challenging to identify the most important zeitgebers. For instance, light is mostly not accessible for the many workers that remain inside the hive. Also, given the fact that temperature is such an important factor for breeding it is tightly regulated. Based on laboratory experiments that suggest that at least 6° to 10° C degrees oscillations are necessary to entrain bees (Moore and Rankin, 1993Moore D and Rankin MA (1993) Light and temperature entrainment of a locomotor rhythm in honeybees. Physiol Entomol 18:271-278.; Fuchikawa and Shimizu, 2007Fuchikawa T and Shimizu I (2007) Circadian rhythm of locomotor activity in the Japanese honeybee, Apis cerana japonica. Physiol Entomol 32:73-80.), the temperature inside the hive is not the most likely factor to serve as a zeitgeber. However, early studies in honeybees in which the investigators entrained bees to different phases and then put them together suggested that bees can socially synchronize to a common activity phase (Moritz and Kryger, 1994Moritz RFA and Kryger P (1994) Self-organization of circadian rhythms in groups of honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 34:211-215.). The authors showed that two groups of bees that were separated by a partition that allowed airflow improved their synchronization to a common daily rhythm. These early results provided a lot of evidence to the hypothesis that volatiles are crucial for social synchronization. Given the fact that the animals were able to touch each other, the role of direct contact was not eliminated. However, by using coloured food, the authors were able to prove that trophallactic (mouth-to-mouth) contacts were not involved in social synchronization given more support to the volatile hypothesis.

In addition to volatiles and trophallaxis, another important socialization mechanism is the direct contact between animals. Nevertheless, experiments performed by different groups suggest that direct contact is not needed for bees to experience social synchronization of activity (Beer et al., 2016Beer K, Steffan-Dewenter I, Härtel S and Helfrich-Förster C (2016) A new device for monitoring individual activity rhythms of honey bees reveals critical effects of the social environment on behavior. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 202:555-565.; Fuchikawa et al., 2016Fuchikawa T, Eban-Rothschild A, Nagari M, Shemesh Y and Bloch G (2016) Potent social synchronization can override photic entrainment of circadian rhythms. Nat Commun 7:11662.). In particular, Fuchikawa et al. (2016Fuchikawa T, Eban-Rothschild A, Nagari M, Shemesh Y and Bloch G (2016) Potent social synchronization can override photic entrainment of circadian rhythms. Nat Commun 7:11662.) performed their experiments with honeybees within the hive with the goal of uncoupling masking from entrainment. These experiments were the first to clearly show that social cues can be as powerful, or even more, than photic cues to entrain the circadian clock of an insect. They show that the social cues of the hive can entrain the clock in animals that are experiencing conflicting information. Exposing newly emerged workers to the colony environment for two days was sufficient to show strong entrainment to the colony phase. The synchronization experienced by the bees in the hive, compared to individually isolated sisters could come from direct interactions with other animals or by a synchronizing effect of the colony environment. To test this, the authors set animals to interact outside the hive or placed the bees in a cage inside the hive precluding physical contact but allowing access to volatile as well as mechanical information. The results of these experiments showed that interaction with conspecifics is important, since the phase coherence of the group of animals that interacted outside the hive improved, but, more importantly, the hive environment is per se capable of synchronizing animals that were isolated inside the hive (Fuchikawa et al., 2016Fuchikawa T, Eban-Rothschild A, Nagari M, Shemesh Y and Bloch G (2016) Potent social synchronization can override photic entrainment of circadian rhythms. Nat Commun 7:11662.).

On the other hand, Beer and colleagues utilized a novel and insightful experimental set up that allows monitoring the activity rhythms of individual honeybees while they are exposed to the social context of a colony (Beer et al., 2016Beer K, Steffan-Dewenter I, Härtel S and Helfrich-Förster C (2016) A new device for monitoring individual activity rhythms of honey bees reveals critical effects of the social environment on behavior. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 202:555-565.). To do this, they placed bees in individually monitored tubes that allowed them to be in contact with environmental cues from the hive. By doing so, they were able to record the activity of individual animals while they are exposed to social cues from the hive. In one experiment the animals were exposed to conflicting zeitgeber cues, i.e. an outdoor colony was transfer to a shifted light-dark (LD) cycle in an incubator. Isolated animals synchronized to the LD cycle faster, whereas animals that were exposed to the signals coming from the hive kept their phase in synchrony and took significantly longer to re-entrain to the new LD set by the incubator light schedule. Additionally, they wanted to evaluate the role of direct contact on social synchronization, and by using an experimental device that separated the animals from the colony by a double mesh, they showed that direct contact is not necessary for honeybees to experience social synchronization (Beer et al., 2016Beer K, Steffan-Dewenter I, Härtel S and Helfrich-Förster C (2016) A new device for monitoring individual activity rhythms of honey bees reveals critical effects of the social environment on behavior. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 202:555-565.). It has also been proposed that other activities performed by the workers could be the source of the social entrainment, such as airborne and substrate-borne vibrations, volatile pheromones, hive odours, or gases such as CO2 or O2 (Moore and Rankin, 1993Moore D and Rankin MA (1993) Light and temperature entrainment of a locomotor rhythm in honeybees. Physiol Entomol 18:271-278.; Fuchikawa and Shimizu, 2007Fuchikawa T and Shimizu I (2007) Circadian rhythm of locomotor activity in the Japanese honeybee, Apis cerana japonica. Physiol Entomol 32:73-80.; Ohashi et al., 2009Ohashi M, Okada R, Kimura T and Ikeno H (2009) Observation system for the control of the hive environment by the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Behav Res Methods 41:782-786.). In particular, studying the relevance of substrate-born vibration it has been shown that bees that were individually housed in chambers within the same tray have higher coupling strength values compared to bees at a similar distance but caged on a different, mechanically isolated, tray. These results clearly show that vibrations can play a role in social synchronization. Additionally, individually caged animals placed in one tray with interconnecting tubes that allows the transfer of volatiles, and also allows the animals to touch and lick each other, had a stronger phase coherence compared to bees on the same tray but housed in unconnected cages. These results also point out the relevance of olfactory cues involved in this process; however, these experiments are not sufficient to rule out the role of direct contact on social synchronization.

A key set of experiment that proves the true entrainment nature of social synchronization in honeybees comes for the work of Siehler and Bloch (2020Siehler O and Bloch G (2020) Colony volatiles and substrate-borne vibrations entrain circadian rhythms and are potential cues mediating social synchronization in honey bee colonies. J Biol Rhythms 35:246-256.). They showed that bees exposed for six days to the volatiles of a foraging hive show a closer phase relationship with foraging bees of the same hive compared with bees exposed to an empty hive of a comparable size. The key contribution is that the rhythmic activity was measured after removing the animals from the environment that was used to entrain them. This shows that properties of the internal clock have been modified, rather than attributing the changes solely to the masking effect of social cues.

In conclusion, social entrainment can be a potent and stable zeitgeber, and even overriding photic entrainment of circadian rhythms in honeybees, challenging again the view that light plays the dominant role in animal circadian entrainment. However, in many contexts we still do not understand if the phase of the circadian clock is truly altered, or whether synchrony and segregation of activity respond to a masking effect, or a learning process based on a circadian time sense. In addition, the question of which are the volatiles that are the synchronizing cues remains. Some authors propose that volatile molecules such as pheromones or fluctuations in the concentration of gases, such as CO2 or O2, that depend on the level of activity of the animals could be the actual signals that convey the social synchronization, but the actual relevant signal(s) remain elusive.

Concluding remarks

Investigating the circadian entrainment by social cues opens avenues for exploring the intricate relationship between an organism’s internal state and its external environment. Unravelling the molecular and neural pathways involved in this process will enhance our comprehension of how animals adapt to their surroundings and optimize their chances of survival.

Additionally, understanding the influence of social information on the circadian clock has implications across various disciplines, including neuroscience, psychology, and chronobiology. Internal misalignment with the external cues due to travelling across time-zones (jetlag), as well as social jetlag, the disruptions in circadian rhythms due to socially imposed schedules, has been associated with adverse health outcomes, including sleep, psychiatric and metabolic disorders. By investigating how social information can entrain the circadian clock, we can develop strategies to mitigate the negative effects of jetlag and optimize the synchronization of our internal clock. These lines of research will foster interdisciplinary discussions and inspire future collaborations aimed at enhancing our understanding of human and animal health, contributing to a more holistic perspective on well-being.

Acknowledgements

We want to thank all the members of the Laboratory of Physiology and Sensory Plasticity at IFIBYNE for the critical discussion and continuous support, Ezequiel Alba Posse for the art in Figure 3, and Nala for constant assistance. ChCP is funded by the fellowship Estímulo a las Vocaciones Científicas CIN, NP is supported by CONICET and Universidad de Buenos Aires, and EJB is supported by CONICET.

References

  • Abraham U, Saleh M and Kramer A (2013) Odor is a time cue for circadian behavior. J Biol Rhythms 28:26-37.
  • Aschoff J (1960) Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 25:11-28.
  • Aschoff J, Fatranska M, Giedke H, Doerr P, Stamm D and Wisser H (1971) Human circadian rhythms in continuous darkness: Entrainment by social cues. Science 171:213-215.
  • Beckwith EJ and Yanovsky MJ (2014) Circadian regulation of gene expression: At the crossroads of transcriptional and post-transcriptional regulatory networks. Curr Opin Genet Dev 27:35-42.
  • Beer K, Steffan-Dewenter I, Härtel S and Helfrich-Förster C (2016) A new device for monitoring individual activity rhythms of honey bees reveals critical effects of the social environment on behavior. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 202:555-565.
  • Bloch G, Herzog ED, Levine JD and Schwartz WJ (2013) Socially synchronized circadian oscillators. Proc Biol Sci 280:20130035.
  • Bovet J and Oertli EF (1974) Free-running circadian activity rhythms in free-living beaver (Castor canadensis). J Comp Physiol 92:1-10.
  • Caldelas I, Gonzalez B, Montufar-Chaveznava R and Hudson R (2009) Endogenous clock gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of previsual newborn rabbits is entrained by nursing. Dev Neurobiol 69:47-59.
  • Chang AM, Santhi N, St Hilaire M, Gronfier C, Bradstreet DS, Duffy JF, Lockley SW, Kronauer RE and Czeisler CA (2012) Human responses to bright light of different durations. J Physiol 590:3103-3112.
  • Cox KH and Takahashi JS (2019) Circadian clock genes and the transcriptional architecture of the clock mechanism. J Mol Endocrinol 63:R93-R102.
  • Crowley M and Bovet J (1980) Social synchronization of circadian rhythms in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:99-105.
  • Das B and de Bekker C (2022) Time-course RNASeq of Camponotus floridanus forager and nurse ant brains indicate links between plasticity in the biological clock and behavioral division of labor. BMC Genomics 23:57.
  • Dibner C, Schibler U and Albrecht U (2010) The mammalian circadian timing system: Organization and coordination of central and peripheral clocks. Annu Rev Physiol 72:517-549.
  • Foster RG, Hughes S and Peirson SN (2020) Circadian photoentrainment in mice and humans. Biology (Basel) 9:180.
  • Fuchikawa T and Shimizu I (2007) Circadian rhythm of locomotor activity in the Japanese honeybee, Apis cerana japonica Physiol Entomol 32:73-80.
  • Fuchikawa T, Eban-Rothschild A, Nagari M, Shemesh Y and Bloch G (2016) Potent social synchronization can override photic entrainment of circadian rhythms. Nat Commun 7:11662.
  • Fujioka H, Abe MS, Fuchikawa T, Tsuji K, Shimada M and Okada Y (2017) Ant circadian activity associated with brood care type. Biol Lett 13:20160743.
  • Fujioka H, Abe MS and Okada Y (2019) Ant activity-rest rhythms vary with age and interaction frequencies of workers. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 73:30.
  • Fujioka H, Abe MS and Okada Y (2021) Individual ants do not show activity-rest rhythms in nest conditions. J Biol Rhythms 36:297-310.
  • Goel N (2006) An arousing, musically enhanced bird song stimulus mediates circadian rhythm phase advances in dim light. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 291:R822-R827.
  • Goel N and Lee TM (1996) Relationship of circadian activity and social behaviors to reentrainment rates in diurnal Octodon degus (Rodentia). Physiol Behav 59:817-826.
  • Goel N and Lee TM (1997) Olfactory bulbectomy impedes social but not photic reentrainment of circadian rhythms in female Octodon degus J Biol Rhythms 12:362-370.
  • Governale MM and Lee TM (2001) Olfactory cues accelerate reentrainment following phase shifts and entrain free-running rhythms in female Octodon degus (Rodentia). J Biol Rhythms 16:489-501.
  • Hagenauer MH and Lee TM (2008) Circadian organization of the diurnal Caviomorph rodent, Octodon degus Biol Rhythm Res 39:269-289.
  • Ingram KK, Krummey S and LeRoux M (2009) Expression patterns of a circadian clock gene are associated with age-related polyethism in harvester ants, Pogonomyrmex occidentalis BMC Ecol 9:7.
  • Krishnan B, Dryer SE and Hardin PE (1999) Circadian rhythms in olfactory responses of Drosophila melanogaster Nature 400:375-378.
  • Krishnan P, Chatterjee A, Tanoue S and Hardin PE (2008) Spike amplitude of single-unit responses in antennal sensillae is controlled by the Drosophila circadian clock. Curr Biol 18:803-807.
  • Krupp JJ, Kent C, Billeter JC, Azanchi R, So AK, Schonfeld JA, Smith BP, Lucas C and Levine JD (2008) Social experience modifies pheromone expression and mating behavior in male Drosophila melanogaster Curr Biol 18:1373-1383.
  • Kumar V (2017) Biological timekeeping: Clocks, rhythms and behaviour. Springer, New Delhi.
  • Levine JD, Funes P, Dowse HB and Hall JC (2002) Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster Science 298:2010-2012.
  • Lone SR and Sharma VK (2011) Social synchronization of circadian locomotor activity rhythm in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster J Exp Biol 214:3742-3750.
  • Marimuthu G, Subbaraj R and Chandrashekaran MK (1978) Social synchronization of the activity rhythm in a cave-dwelling insectivorous bat. Naturwissenschaften 65:600.
  • Marimuthu G, Rajan S and Chandrashekaran MK (1981) Social entrainment of the circadian rhythm in the flight activity of the microchiropteran bat Hipposideros speoris Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:147-150.
  • Maywood ES, Smith E, Hall SJ and Hastings MH (1997) A thalamic contribution to arousal-induced, non-photic entrainment of the circadian clock of the Syrian hamster. Eur J Neurosci 9:1739-1747.
  • Menaker M and Eskin A (1966) Entrainment of circadian rhythms by sound in Passer domesticus Science 154:1579-1581.
  • Meyer N, Harvey AG, Lockley SW and Dijk DJ (2022) Circadian rhythms and disorders of the timing of sleep. Lancet 400:1061-1078.
  • Mistlberger RE and Antle MC (2011) Entrainment of circadian clocks in mammals by arousal and food. Essays Biochem 49:119-136.
  • Montufar-Chaveznava R, Trejo-Munoz L, Hernandez-Campos O, Navarrete E and Caldelas I (2013) Maternal olfactory cues synchronize the circadian system of artificially raised newborn rabbits. PLoS One 8:e74048.
  • Moore D and Rankin MA (1993) Light and temperature entrainment of a locomotor rhythm in honeybees. Physiol Entomol 18:271-278.
  • Moritz RFA and Kryger P (1994) Self-organization of circadian rhythms in groups of honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 34:211-215.
  • Mrosovsky N (1988) Phase response curves for social entrainment. J Comp Physiol A 162:35-46.
  • Mrosovsky N (1999) Masking: History, definitions, and measurement. Chronobiol Int 16:415-429.
  • Ohashi M, Okada R, Kimura T and Ikeno H (2009) Observation system for the control of the hive environment by the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Behav Res Methods 41:782-786.
  • Olejnikova L, Polidarova L, Behuliak M, Sladek M and Sumova A (2018) Circadian alignment in a foster mother improves the offspring’s pathological phenotype. J Physiol 596:5757-5775.
  • Reinhard N, Fukuda A, Manoli G, Derksen E, Saito A, Möller G, Sekiguchi M, Rieger D, Helfrich-Förster C, Yoshii T et al (2023) Synaptic and peptidergic connectomes of the Drosophila circadian clock. bioRxiv: 2023.09.11.557222.
  • Rijo-Ferreira F and Takahashi JS (2019) Genomics of circadian rhythms in health and disease. Genome Med 11:82.
  • Sack RL, Lewy AJ, Blood ML, Keith LD and Nakagawa H (1992) Circadian rhythm abnormalities in totally blind people: incidence and clinical significance. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 75:127-134.
  • Seeley TD (1995) The wisdom of the hive: The social physiology of honey bee colonies. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
  • Sehadova H, Glaser FT, Gentile C, Simoni A, Giesecke A, Albert JT and Stanewsky R (2009) Temperature entrainment of Drosophila’s circadian clock involves the gene nocte and signaling from peripheral sensory tissues to the brain. Neuron 64:251-266.
  • Shafer OT, Gutierrez GJ, Li K, Mildenhall A, Spira D, Marty J, Lazar AA and Fernandez MP (2022) Connectomic analysis of the Drosophila lateral neuron clock cells reveals the synaptic basis of functional pacemaker classes. Elife 11:e79139.
  • Siehler O and Bloch G (2020) Colony volatiles and substrate-borne vibrations entrain circadian rhythms and are potential cues mediating social synchronization in honey bee colonies. J Biol Rhythms 35:246-256.
  • Simoni A, Wolfgang W, Topping MP, Kavlie RG, Stanewsky R and Albert JT (2014) A mechanosensory pathway to the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 343:525-528.
  • Sonker P and Singaravel M (2021) Gender difference in circadian clock responses for social interaction with conspecific of the opposite-sex. Chronobiol Int 38:212-223.
  • Sonker P, Cherukalady R and Singaravel M (2021) Effect of periodic social interaction and odour presentation of same and opposite-sex conspecifics on free-running mice. Chronobiol Int 38:1714-1725.
  • Starnes AN and Jones JR (2023) Inputs and outputs of the mammalian circadian clock. Biology 12:508.
  • Sumova A, Bendova Z, Sladek M, El-Hennamy R, Laurinova K, Jindrakova Z and Illnerova H (2006) Setting the biological time in central and peripheral clocks during ontogenesis. FEBS Lett 580:2836-2842.
  • Tanoue S, Krishnan P, Krishnan B, Dryer SE and Hardin PE (2004) Circadian clocks in antennal neurons are necessary and sufficient for olfaction rhythms in Drosophila Curr Biol 14:638-649.
  • Viswanathan N and Chandrashekaran MK (1985) Cycles of presence and absence of mother mouse entrain the circadian clock of pups. Nature 317:530-531.
  • Webb IC, Antle MC and Mistlberger RE (2014) Regulation of circadian rhythms in mammals by behavioral arousal. Behav Neurosci 128:304-325.
  • Yamakawa GR, Basu P, Cortese F, MacDonnell J, Whalley D, Smith VM and Antle MC (2016) The cholinergic forebrain arousal system acts directly on the circadian pacemaker. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:13498-13503
  • *
    These authors contributed equally to this work

Edited by

Associate Editor:

Carlos F. M. Menck

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    22 July 2024
  • Date of issue
    2024

History

  • Received
    30 Jan 2024
  • Accepted
    10 May 2024
Sociedade Brasileira de Genética Rua Cap. Adelmio Norberto da Silva, 736, 14025-670 Ribeirão Preto SP Brazil, Tel.: (55 16) 3911-4130 / Fax.: (55 16) 3621-3552 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: editor@gmb.org.br