Abstract
Introduction
Interpersonal negotiation skills (INS) comprise actions used to solve social situations between interacting individuals involving different needs or desires. These abilities are part of one’s social competence and may be impaired in some psychiatric conditions. There are few validated psychometric tools for measuring INS in the literature. This pilot study aimed to investigate some basic psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the Interpersonal Negotiation Strategies Interview (INSI) in children and adolescents.
Methods
We developed a new version of the INSI adapted to the Brazilian culture using eight different dilemmas in dyadic situations (with peers and adults), presented visually as drawings on cards. A group of psychologists and psychiatrists chose and adapted the dilemmas formerly proposed by the original version. The same scoring criteria as for the original instrument were used. A total of 20 children and adolescents were included in this pilot study. We investigated test reliability using measures of interrater reliability, test-retest, and internal consistency. The content validity of the INSI was also evaluated by comparison with scores from the Child Behavior Checklist-Revised (CBCL).
Results
Internal consistency and test-retest evaluations were acceptable (rater 1: α = 0.77; rater 2: α = 0.72); the reliability of the instrument was excellent (K = 0.078; intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.98; 95% confidence interval 0.97-0.99); and content validity was strongly significant (p < 0.001).
Conclusions
Preliminary results suggest that this version of the INSI has good interrater reliability and internal consistency and constitutes a promising tool to assess social competence.
Social skills; psychometrics; pilot projects; social issues
Introduction
Social functioning refers to a comprehensive concept that includes social competence in some domains of life as a defining variable. Social competence comprises the individual’s repertoire of communication skills and interpersonal negotiation skills (INS). INS enable an individual to develop and maintain good interpersonal relationships, express prosocial behavioral patterns, and achieve good acceptance by peers and people from other age groups.11. Selman R, Beardslee W, Schultz L, Krupa M, Podorefsky D. Assessing adolescent interpersonal negotiation strategies: toward the integration of structural and functional models. Dev Psychol. 1986;22:450-9.
2. Walker HM, Ramsay E, Gresham FM. Antisocial behavior in school: evidence-based practices. Belmont: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning; 2004.-33. Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Spiegel JA, Day TN, Harmon SL, Wells EL. Heterogeneity in ADHD: Neurocognitive predictors of peer, family, and academic functioning. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23:733-59. INS comprise actions used to solve conflicts evoked when one’s needs or desires disagree with those of others.44. Yeates KO, Selman RL. Social competence in the schools: toward an integrative developmental model for intervention. Dev Rev. 1989;9:64-100. This behavior is preceded by cognitive operations comprising perception, interpretation, and response elaboration, considering one’s intention and possible outcomes from interactions with partners.55. Fiske ST, Taylor SE. Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill Education; 1991.
Use of deficient strategies to negotiate interpersonal conflicts is a critical feature of problematic relationships and strongly contributes to social impairment.66. Lochman JE, Wayland K. Aggression, social acceptance, and race as predictors of negative adolescent outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33:1026-35. The ability to negotiate successful solutions to social conflicts is a cognitive skill that develops throughout pre-adolescence and early adolescence, hypothesized as a precursor to moral development and prosocial behaviors.77. Kurdek LA. Perspective taking as the cognitive basis of children’s moral development: a review of the literature. Merrill Palmer Q. 1978;24:3-28. This ability is positively related to empathic understanding, helping behaviors, social competence, and social status.88. Uekovik M, Gerris, IRM. Developmental analysis of social cognitive and behavioural differences between popular and rejected children. J Appl Develop Psychol. 1994;15:367-86.
9. Underwood B, Moore B. Perspective taking and altruism. Psychol Bull. 1982;91:143-73.
10. Leadbeater BJ, Hellner I, Allen JP, Aber JL. Assessment of interpersonal negotiation strategies in youth engaged in problem behaviors. Dev Psychol. 1989;2:465-72.-1111. Yeates KO, Shultz LH, Selman RL. The development of interpersonal negotiation strategies in thought and action: a social-cognitive link to behavioral adjustment and social status. Merrill-Palmer Q. 1991;37:369-405.
Using observational and interview procedures, Selman et al. proposed the interpersonal negotiation strategies (INS) model of how children and adolescents articulate and use strategies to negotiate interpersonal conflicts.1212. Abrahami A, Selman RL, Stone C. A developmental assessment of children’s verbal strategies for social action resolution. J Appl Devel Psychology. 1981;2:145-63.,1313. Selman RL, Demorest AP. Observing troubled children’s interpersonal negotiation strategies: implications of and for a developmental model. Child Dev. 1984;55:288-304. This model focuses on how, in a dyadic context for interpersonal negotiation, one individual deals with another to resolve the disequilibrium that arises between them in particular social conflict situations. The model takes into consideration that context influences negotiation. An adolescent may use a different overall level of interpersonal negotiation with a peer than he/she would use with an adult. It also includes some situations in which the protagonist is attempting a change (initiation), and other situations in which the protagonist is reacting to a change initiated by the other person (reaction). Situations may also vary in terms of whether the other individual has an intimate relationship with the protagonist or not.
The INS model proposes that solving a social dilemma comprises four main stages: 1) comprehension/identification of the problem; 2) feelings triggered by the social problem-solving process 3) choosing the strategy to be used; and 4) justification (prospecting outcomes). The pattern of responses presented in each of these steps is the variable to be assessed. This pattern is classified according to the level of complexity of the ability to coordinate conflicting individual perspectives. The original model considers four levels of complexity: impulsive, unilateral, reciprocal, and collaborative. It is expected that the level of complexity will progressively evolve throughout the stages of development, already reaching a satisfactory level during adolescence.1111. Yeates KO, Shultz LH, Selman RL. The development of interpersonal negotiation strategies in thought and action: a social-cognitive link to behavioral adjustment and social status. Merrill-Palmer Q. 1991;37:369-405. Based on the INS theoretical model, the Interpersonal Negotiation Strategies Interview (INSI) is a psychometric instrument developed by Selman et al.11. Selman R, Beardslee W, Schultz L, Krupa M, Podorefsky D. Assessing adolescent interpersonal negotiation strategies: toward the integration of structural and functional models. Dev Psychol. 1986;22:450-9. It is a structured dilemma-discussion interview involving hypothetical social conflict situations used to assess the four steps of the INS process (Table 1).11. Selman R, Beardslee W, Schultz L, Krupa M, Podorefsky D. Assessing adolescent interpersonal negotiation strategies: toward the integration of structural and functional models. Dev Psychol. 1986;22:450-9. In the original instrument, dilemmas are presented verbally to the examinee.
Since its creation and validation, the INSI has been used as a psychometric tool to assess INS in different samples. Social dilemmas have sometimes been modified according to the goals of the study. Some authors have used pictures and even videos to portray dyadic social situations.1414. Jennings TC, Hulbert CA, Jackson HJ, Chanen AM. Social perspective coordination in youth with borderline personality pathology. J Pers Disord. 2012;26:126-40.
15. Rosenthal SL, Cohen SS, Biro FM. Developmental sophistication among adolescents of negotiation strategies for condom use. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1996;17:94-7.-1616. Taylor SA. Social functioning of children and adolescents with ADHD: communication functioning and social problem solving as possible underlying mechanisms [dissertation]. Mount Pleasant: University of Otago; 2009. However, scoring criteria have always followed the original proposal for assessment of four steps according to the original INS model.1717. Goldstein TR, Miklowitz DJ, Mullen KL. Social skills knowledge and performance among adolescents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2016;8:350-61.
Children and adolescents who have or are at risk of psychiatric disorders often experience difficulties in social competences; these difficulties should be investigated by mental health professionals because of their potential adverse outcomes. Social competence in children and adolescents is widely assessed by collateral informants, mainly parents and teachers; while admittedly important, this strategy can, however, be heavily influenced by a myriad of biases.1818. Maag JW. Social skills training for youth with emotional and behavioral disorders and learning disabilities: problems, conclusions, and suggestions. Exceptionality. 2005;13:155-72.
19. Maag JW. Social skills training for students with emotional and behavioral disorders: a review of reviews. Behav Disord. 2006;32:155-72.-2020. Renk, K. Cross-informant ratings of social competence in children and adolescents. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;24:239-54. Also, interventions for impairments in social functioning generally involve strategies based on the assumption that the person did not acquire the social skills along their developmental stages.2121. Tseng WL, Kawabata Y, Gau SS, Crick NR. Symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and peer functioning: a transactional model of development. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42:1353-65. However, collateral report may not be informative for subgroups of children with social deficits that are mediated by specific conditions such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Some childhood psychiatric conditions are associated with performance deficits rather than with skills acquisition deficits.2222. de Boo GM, Prins PJ. Social incompetence in children with ADHD: possible moderators and mediators in social-skills training. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27:78-97.,2323. Mikami AY. Social skills training for youth with ADHD. In: Barkley RA, ed. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. 4th ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2014. p. 569-95.
Considering the relevance and scarcity of standard psychometric tools that assess social competence domains (social skills knowledge and performance), we consider the INSI a promising assessment tool for both research and practice. We believe that the Brazilian version of the INSI can open new perspectives for evaluation of these abilities in this particular culture. In this pilot study, we report the psychometric properties (internal consistency, reliability, and validity) of the Brazilian version of the INSI in a sample of patients with varied conditions from a children’s psychiatric center.
Methods
Participants
A total of 20 participants were examined (children = 13, age range 8-12 years-old; adolescents = 7, age range 13-17 years-old). All of them were recruited through the outpatient unit for neuropsychological assessment at the Instituto D’Or de Ensino e Pesquisa (IDOR), in Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. All participants had been referred because of learning and/or behavioral problems and underwent a sequential order of evaluation that comprised psychiatric assessment, INSI interview, and neuropsychological and language assessments. Participants had middle-high socioeconomic status according to the occupation and education level of their parents (Hollingshead AB, Four-factor index of social status, unpublished manuscript, New Haven, Yale University, 1975). This study was approved by the IDOR ethics committee. All participants and their respective parents provided written informed consent.
Exclusion criteria were: a) cognitive impairment defined as an intelligence quotient (IQ) < 80; b) presence of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and/or autistic traits; c) communication disorders (language development disorder); d) psychosis or acute mania; or e) genetic syndromes or neurological conditions (e.g., epilepsy). All interviews were conducted by a board-certified psychiatrist using criteria from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5).2424. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
Measures
Child and adolescent psychiatric evaluation
A semi-structured clinical interview was conducted with the participants’ parents using the adapted version of the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Aged Children (K-SADS). The K-SADS (2017 update) was used to assess the nature, onset, course, duration, severity, and impairment of current and past psychopathology episodes in the children and adolescents included in the sample, according to DSM-5 criteria.2424. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. The Brazilian version of the K-SADS has shown good convergent validity with the original version.2525. Caye A, Kieling RR, Rocha TB, Graeff-Martins AS, Geyer C, Krieger F, et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children - Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL), DSM-5 update: translation into Brazilian Portuguese. Braz J Psychiatry. 2017;39:384-6.
Parents provided a collateral report regarding anxiety and ADHD symptoms through the parent’s version of Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) and the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV Questionnaire (SNAP-IV), respectively.2626. Biaggio AMB, Spielberger CD. Inventário de ansiedade Traço-Estado-IDATE-C- Manual para a forma experimental infantil em português. Rio de Janeiro: Centro Editor de Psicologia Aplicada (CEPA); 1983.,2727. Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde LA, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28:3. The Portuguese version of the STAI has shown high internal consistency, with Cronbach’s α = 0.89. The SNAP-IV was developed to assess symptoms of ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) according to DSM-IV diagnostic criteria.
All participants completed the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) and the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) to investigate the presence of anxiety and depressive symptoms, respectively.2828. Barbosa GA, Gaião e Barbosa A, Gouveia VV. Transtorno de ansiedade na infância e adolescência: um estudo de prevalência e validação de um instrumento (SCARED) de triagem. INFANTO. 2002;10:34-47.,2929. Wathier JL, Dell’Aglio DB, Bandeira, DR. Análise fatorial do Inventário de Depressão Infantil (CDI) em amostra de jovens brasileiros. Aval Psicol. 2008;1;75-84. Similarly to the original version, the Portuguese version of SCARED comprises 69 items that aim to assess different dimensions of anxiety-related problems in children: separation anxiety, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, social phobia, specific phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and post-traumatic stress. The CDI is internationally considered as the most widely used instrument to assess depressive symptoms in children and adolescents, in both clinical and research contexts.
Social problem evaluation
Parents filled out the Child Behavior Checklist-Revised (CBCL) in order to record social problems.3030. Bordin IA, Mari JJ, Caeiro MF. Validação da versão brasileira do “Child Behavior Checklist” (CBCL) - inventário de comportamento da infância e da adolescência: dados preliminares. Rev ABP-APAL. 1995;2:55-66. Based on previous exploratory analyses, using the items from the CBCL Social Problem Scale, it was possible to assess two domains of social functioning “Social Immaturity” and “Peer Rejection.”3131. Carpenter Rich E, Loo SK, Yang M, Dang J, Smalley SL. Social functioning difficulties in ADHD: association with PDD risk. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;14:329-44. We opted to use correlations between four items of the “Peer Rejection” subscale (25, 37, 38, and 48) and INSI scores in further analyses, considering the close relationship between Interpersonal Negotiation ability and outcomes related to Peer Acceptance in social functioning assessment.3232. Masten AS. Peer relationships and psychopathology in developmental perspective: reflections on progress and promise. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychology. 2005;34:87-92.
Cognitive assessment and language evaluation
In order to exclude primary comprehension bias, such as cognitive and/or communication deficits, the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WISC-IV) was administered to participants for IQ measurement. Also, the Brazilian versions of the Protocole Montréal d`Évaluation de la Communication and the Faux Pas and Teste de Coerência e Inferência Local were administered to all participants to assess language development and social cognition features.3333. Wagner F, Camey SA, Trentini CM. Análise fatorial confirmatória da escala de inteligência Wechsler abreviada: versão português brasileiro. Aval Psicol. 2014;13:383-9.
34. Fonseca RP, Parente MA, Cote H, Ska B, Joanette Y. Bateria MAC - Bateria Montreal de avaliação da comunicação. São Paulo: Pro-Fono; 2008.-3535. Faísca L, Afonseca S, Brüne M, Gonçalves G, Gomes A, Martins AT. Portuguese adaptation of a Faux Pas Test and a theory of mind picture stories task. Psychopathology. 2016;15:17.
The Brazilian version of the INSI and scoring: identification of domains and construction of dilemmas
Four psychiatrists and two psychologists with extensive experience in children’s and adolescents’ mental health were invited to participate in two separate meetings to develop the social dilemmas to be used in the structured interview. At the first meeting, four dilemmas for evaluation of children (8-12 years old) and four for evaluation of adolescents (13-17 years old) were chosen from the twelve dilemmas from the original version of the INSI (covering seven situations involving adolescents and five involving children).11. Selman R, Beardslee W, Schultz L, Krupa M, Podorefsky D. Assessing adolescent interpersonal negotiation strategies: toward the integration of structural and functional models. Dev Psychol. 1986;22:450-9. Criteria for selection of dilemmas included the need to choose two dilemmas portraying social situations involving peers and two dilemmas involving cases with adults. For each of the relationship domains, one situation should represent an intimate relationship. The professionals’ choice of dilemmas was based on their individual judgment of which dilemmas were most congruent with the Brazilian social culture (for example, dilemmas that refer to work situations for adolescents were excluded, since middle-income adolescents do not work in Brazil). There were no ties in dilemma selection, although it had been agreed in advance that, in the event of a tie, the first author would vote for a tiebreaker.
Once the dilemmas had been selected, they were adapted to the Brazilian culture. The texts for the hypothetical dilemmas were not written by translation from the English, but utilized the primary nuclei of each history (a dyadic social problem). The final versions of each of the dilemmas used in the interview are presented in the Appendix and available as online-only supplementary material.
At the second meeting, the scoring criteria to debrief the four steps of evaluation were discussed together with their corresponding scoring criteria adapted from the original INS model. Following the original model, responses should be scored on a 4-point scale (0-3) for each step. The scoring criteria are shown in Table 2. The sum of the total score represents the global INS index (INSI total score).
The INSI application included reading the short history of each dilemma and asking semi-structured questions regarding problem-solving steps. The dilemmas were presented in the same order for all participants. The protagonist in each dilemma always had the same gender as the participant (there were always two options for each dilemma). We used cards portraying the dilemmas to mitigate attentional or working memory deficits (example in Figure 1). Each scene was described aloud by the examiner while presenting the card to the examinee. Answers were audio-recorded and transcribed. The total score of the Brazilian version of INSI was used for subsequent analysis.
Example of a card portraying one of the dilemmas for children. Dilemma: “Felipe/Maria Clara is attending a class with a substitute teacher. He/she needs to leave earlier today because he/she has an important appointment. But he/she realizes that he/she has forgotten the authorization signed by his/her mother. When he/she approaches his/her teacher, she says Felipe/Maria Clara must have the document in order to leave, because this is the school’s rule.”
Data collection and analysis
The participants were enrolled on a non-randomized sequential order of evaluation. The total evaluation time lasted three days, with appointments lasting between 2 and 4 hours. The first day included the psychiatric assessment, collection of participant’s and parents’ reports (using the questionnaires), and the INSI interview. On the second day, the participant underwent the neuropsychological tests. On the last day, the participant underwent the language assessment.
Two previously trained interviewers participated in collecting the INSI data. Two blinded raters (designated raters 1 and 2) scored the INSI responses for the four steps, each providing total scores for the same subjects independently. Raters had separate meetings with two authors (TF and PM) in order to clarify items and check for consistency using real examples from the sample. The total score of the Brazilian version of the INSI was used for subsequent analysis.
The normality of the data distribution was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. The internal consistency was estimated using Cronbach’s α and values larger than 0.70 were considered acceptable.3636. George D, Mallery P. SPSS for Windows step by step: a simple guide and reference. 11.0 update. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2002. Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC [2,1]) and the Kappa (K) concordance test were applied to measure interrater and test-retest reliability, respectively.3737. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15:155-63. These results were interpreted as follows: < 0.5 = poor reliability, between 0.5 and 0.75 = moderate reliability, between 0.75 and 0.9 = good reliability, and > 0.90 = excellent reliability.3737. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15:155-63. The significance level was set at α=0.05. In addition, measurement error was evaluated using standard error of measurement (SEM), as follows: SEM = SD x Ö(1-ICC); where: SD = Standard deviation.3838. Polit DF. Getting serious about test–retest reliability: a critique of retest research and some recommendations. Qual Life Res. 2014;23:1713-20. The results for Internal Consistency, ICC, and test-retest were used to evaluate the reliability of the Brazilian Version of the INSI.
The scores of CBCL items that assess social problems were used to test the content validity of the INSI. Construct validity was investigated by examining the relationship between the total INSI score and the CBCL score. Spearman’s correlation coefficients were used to investigate correlations between INSI and CBCL scores. Due to a need to calculate the number of participants for an additional analysis involving ADHD-related social problems, we separated participants into two groups: the ADHD group and the Clinical Control group. Next, we conducted a comparison between mean INSI performance (total scores) of both groups (ADHD and clinical controls) using Pearson’s t test. The IBM Statistical Package for Social Sciences version 25 (SPSS v. 5), Stata 16 software (Stata Corporation), and MS Excel 365 were used for all these analyses.
Results
Sample characteristics
The total sample included twenty children and adolescents. Thirteen (65%) were male. The mean age and IQ of participants was 11.76 (SD = 2.86) years and 104.38 (SD = 11.43), respectively. None of the participants presented language impairment or abnormal performance in Theory of Mind (ToM) tests. Thirteen subjects fulfilled ADHD diagnostic criteria. One of these had comorbid ODD symptoms and nine presented high levels of anxiety. Six of the remaining seven participants had learning disorders and one had a mood disorder. The sociodemographic characteristics and prevalence of psychiatric problems are shown in Table 3.
Internal consistency and interrater reliability
For both raters, internal consistency was acceptable (rater 1: α = 0.77; rater 2: α = 0.72). Results for internal consistency are shown in Table 4. The Kappa (K) agreement coefficient for total INSI score was 0.786 (p < 0.001). The overall reliability of the instrument was substantial (ICC = 0.98; 95% confidence interval [95%CI] 0.97, 0.99). Most of the subitems presented moderate reliability, but some items yielded low ICC values (Comprehension of dilemma item 2: ICC = 0.40; 95%CI -0.48, 0.75; Strategy of situation item 2: ICC = 0.30; 95%CI -0.68, 0.71; Comprehension of situation item 3: ICC = 0.19; 95%CI -1.00, 0.67; and Strategy of situation item 4: ICC = 0.28; 95%CI -0.74, 0.71). On average, reliability indices for comprehension (ICC = 0.63; 95%CI 0.09, 0.85) and emotions (ICC = 0.58; 95%CI -0.02, 0.83) items were lower than for the other INSI subtests. Table 4 shows the results for each dilemma.
Validity
Construct validity (social problems) was analyzed using the total score for the “Peer Rejection” subscale of the CBCL. This was assessed as the correlation between the INSI total score and the correspondent value for the sum of specific CBCL item scores. The INSI total score was found to be robustly, significantly, and inversely correlated with the “peer rejection” subscale of the CBCL (high INSI scores were associated with fewer social problems in parents’ reports), r = -0.86, p < 0.001.
INS scores between the two groups and sample size reliability
Regarding INSI performance, the mean total score in the sample analyzed was 25.3 (SD = 6.81). The mean INSI total scores were 22.72 (SD = 6.04) in the ADHD group and 29.5 (SD =5.95) in the clinical controls group. The difference in INSI performance between the two groups was statistically significant [t(17.3) = -2.69, p < 0.05].
The power of the sample size was analyzed separating the subjects into two groups: the ADHD group and the clinical controls group. A t test was performed considering the mean INSI performance scores of both groups. The significance was α = 0.10 and the test power was 0.80.
Discussion
This study aimed to report the psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the INSI, and preliminary findings obtained in the sample analyzed. This pilot study is part of a larger research project on social competence of children and adolescents and evaluates the performance of an ADHD subset of the sample. Our results suggest that the Brazilian version of the INSI has good interrater reliability, internal consistency, and strongly significant validity with another validated instrument that assesses child and adolescents’ social problems. Preliminary findings indicated deficient performance on INSI in the ADHD group when compared with the comparison group’s performance.
Social competence refers to a broad concept that involves a spectrum of social skills. Among them, the ability to solve interpersonal conflicts involves some social-cognitive skills such as problem recognition (definition), identification of own behavior and others’ intentions and emotions, development of strategies to solve the conflict and managing the immediate and later outcomes. Development of INS during pre-adolescence and adolescence is an essential part of social competence. This process gives individuals an adaptative transition in the way they deal with others, from a position of self-interest to one of collaboration with another for the sake of mutual interest and intimacy.
Children and youth who have or are at risk of some psychiatric disorders experience critical difficulties in developing and maintaining good interpersonal relationships.1919. Maag JW. Social skills training for students with emotional and behavioral disorders: a review of reviews. Behav Disord. 2006;32:155-72. This fact underscores the need for continuous improvement in the assessment of social competence difficulties. An important theoretical perspective refers to a distinction between deficits in acquisition of social skills and deficits in performance of social skills.3939. Gresham FM. Assessment of children’s social skills. J School Psychology. 1981;19:120-34. Consideration of these perspectives enables the nature of specific social skills difficulties to be assessed in order to design specific treatment strategies. A wide number of authors currently use social problem rating scales to evaluate child social competence and measure their social functioning deficits by multi-informant reports (parents and teachers). Although some specific scales address social competence in children, some studies highlight significant discrepancies between multi-informant scores.4040. John K, Gammon GD, Prusoff BA, Warner V. The Social Adjustment Inventory for Children and Adolescents (SAICA): testing for a new semistructured interview. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26:898-911.
41. Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social skills improvement system: rating scales. Bloomington: Pearson Assessments; 2008.
42. Fernández-Jaén A, Fernández-Mayoralas DM, López-Arribas S, Pardos-Véglia A, Muñiz-Borrega B, García-Savaté C, et al. Social and leadership abilities in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: relation with cognitive-attentional capacities. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2012;40:136-46.-4343. Aduen PA, Day TN, Kofler MJ, Harmon SL, Wells EL, Sarver DE. Social problems in ADHD: is it a skills acquisition or performance problem? J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2018;40:440-51. We recognize the importance of the family and teachers’ perceptions of child social competence for assessment. Still, we believe that these reports make a greater contribution to measuring the impairment level than to revealing the target of individual deficits.
The INSI constitutes a dynamic assessment tool that disentangles the different steps involved in interpersonal negotiation, including different perspectives on hypothetical social situations. It demands that the child selects the social skills required and coordinates the best way to use them, considering possible future outcomes, in different social contexts. We believe that this type of direct evaluation allows complementary assessment of both domains of social abilities (social skills acquisition and performance) and can be useful to create individual approach strategies. Moreover, use of quantitative analyses on four different, although related, levels (definition of the problem, action taken, justification and consequences of the strategy chosen, and complexity of feelings expressed) allows an in-depth insight into the individual’s competence.
The Brazilian version of the INSI can be considered an instrument with adequate psychometric properties for use. The internal consistency was moderate in some specific items. The sample assessed was characterized as having a significant number of subjects with ADHD. We believe that the heterogeneity and the small sample size may have contributed to the moderate internal consistency. Also, the test’s inherent properties are not linear due to the different hypothetical situations presented. The statistically significant difference in INSI scores between the ADHD subgroup and clinical controls reinforced the construct validity of impaired social performance. These preliminary results support the need to expand the sample under analysis in order to obtain new findings that contribute to better understanding of the mechanisms involved in the social competence of children and adolescents, especially those with neurodevelopmental disorders.
We did not include individuals with ASD because their significant impairment in ToM would jeopardize analysis of their negotiation strategies. However, we must investigate the ToM measures as covariates in future studies with larger samples, since ToM is an essential part of social cognition and, therefore, an important part of negotiation abilities.4444. Fernandez-Sotos P, Navarro E, Torio I, Dompablo M, Fernandez-Caballero A, Rodriguez-Jimenez R. Pharmacological interventions in social cognition deficits: a systematic mapping review. Psychiatry Res. 2018;270:57-67.,4545. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Harvey PD. Social cognition psychometric evaluation: results of the initial psychometric study. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:494-504. Since this was a pilot study, a small sample was analyzed. The ADHD group’s deficient performance in the INSI constitutes a preliminary finding, and we intend to replicate and deepen this analysis in further studies.
Limitations and further perspectives
Our study has some limitations inherent to a pilot study. Regarding sample characteristics, we analyzed a convenience sample and our small sample size warrants caution in the interpretation of results, but these are similar to other studies.4545. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Harvey PD. Social cognition psychometric evaluation: results of the initial psychometric study. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:494-504. Further analysis should include test-retest processes and possible correlations with intelligence level, gender, and age, demanding a much larger sample. Ultimately, we believe that the majority of ADHD subjects in the sample enrolled in this analysis can be considered a confounding factor in the analysis of the test’s convergent validity.
References
-
1Selman R, Beardslee W, Schultz L, Krupa M, Podorefsky D. Assessing adolescent interpersonal negotiation strategies: toward the integration of structural and functional models. Dev Psychol. 1986;22:450-9.
-
2Walker HM, Ramsay E, Gresham FM. Antisocial behavior in school: evidence-based practices. Belmont: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning; 2004.
-
3Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Spiegel JA, Day TN, Harmon SL, Wells EL. Heterogeneity in ADHD: Neurocognitive predictors of peer, family, and academic functioning. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23:733-59.
-
4Yeates KO, Selman RL. Social competence in the schools: toward an integrative developmental model for intervention. Dev Rev. 1989;9:64-100.
-
5Fiske ST, Taylor SE. Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill Education; 1991.
-
6Lochman JE, Wayland K. Aggression, social acceptance, and race as predictors of negative adolescent outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33:1026-35.
-
7Kurdek LA. Perspective taking as the cognitive basis of children’s moral development: a review of the literature. Merrill Palmer Q. 1978;24:3-28.
-
8Uekovik M, Gerris, IRM. Developmental analysis of social cognitive and behavioural differences between popular and rejected children. J Appl Develop Psychol. 1994;15:367-86.
-
9Underwood B, Moore B. Perspective taking and altruism. Psychol Bull. 1982;91:143-73.
-
10Leadbeater BJ, Hellner I, Allen JP, Aber JL. Assessment of interpersonal negotiation strategies in youth engaged in problem behaviors. Dev Psychol. 1989;2:465-72.
-
11Yeates KO, Shultz LH, Selman RL. The development of interpersonal negotiation strategies in thought and action: a social-cognitive link to behavioral adjustment and social status. Merrill-Palmer Q. 1991;37:369-405.
-
12Abrahami A, Selman RL, Stone C. A developmental assessment of children’s verbal strategies for social action resolution. J Appl Devel Psychology. 1981;2:145-63.
-
13Selman RL, Demorest AP. Observing troubled children’s interpersonal negotiation strategies: implications of and for a developmental model. Child Dev. 1984;55:288-304.
-
14Jennings TC, Hulbert CA, Jackson HJ, Chanen AM. Social perspective coordination in youth with borderline personality pathology. J Pers Disord. 2012;26:126-40.
-
15Rosenthal SL, Cohen SS, Biro FM. Developmental sophistication among adolescents of negotiation strategies for condom use. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1996;17:94-7.
-
16Taylor SA. Social functioning of children and adolescents with ADHD: communication functioning and social problem solving as possible underlying mechanisms [dissertation]. Mount Pleasant: University of Otago; 2009.
-
17Goldstein TR, Miklowitz DJ, Mullen KL. Social skills knowledge and performance among adolescents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2016;8:350-61.
-
18Maag JW. Social skills training for youth with emotional and behavioral disorders and learning disabilities: problems, conclusions, and suggestions. Exceptionality. 2005;13:155-72.
-
19Maag JW. Social skills training for students with emotional and behavioral disorders: a review of reviews. Behav Disord. 2006;32:155-72.
-
20Renk, K. Cross-informant ratings of social competence in children and adolescents. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;24:239-54.
-
21Tseng WL, Kawabata Y, Gau SS, Crick NR. Symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and peer functioning: a transactional model of development. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42:1353-65.
-
22de Boo GM, Prins PJ. Social incompetence in children with ADHD: possible moderators and mediators in social-skills training. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27:78-97.
-
23Mikami AY. Social skills training for youth with ADHD. In: Barkley RA, ed. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. 4th ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2014. p. 569-95.
-
24American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
-
25Caye A, Kieling RR, Rocha TB, Graeff-Martins AS, Geyer C, Krieger F, et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children - Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL), DSM-5 update: translation into Brazilian Portuguese. Braz J Psychiatry. 2017;39:384-6.
-
26Biaggio AMB, Spielberger CD. Inventário de ansiedade Traço-Estado-IDATE-C- Manual para a forma experimental infantil em português. Rio de Janeiro: Centro Editor de Psicologia Aplicada (CEPA); 1983.
-
27Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde LA, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28:3.
-
28Barbosa GA, Gaião e Barbosa A, Gouveia VV. Transtorno de ansiedade na infância e adolescência: um estudo de prevalência e validação de um instrumento (SCARED) de triagem. INFANTO. 2002;10:34-47.
-
29Wathier JL, Dell’Aglio DB, Bandeira, DR. Análise fatorial do Inventário de Depressão Infantil (CDI) em amostra de jovens brasileiros. Aval Psicol. 2008;1;75-84.
-
30Bordin IA, Mari JJ, Caeiro MF. Validação da versão brasileira do “Child Behavior Checklist” (CBCL) - inventário de comportamento da infância e da adolescência: dados preliminares. Rev ABP-APAL. 1995;2:55-66.
-
31Carpenter Rich E, Loo SK, Yang M, Dang J, Smalley SL. Social functioning difficulties in ADHD: association with PDD risk. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;14:329-44.
-
32Masten AS. Peer relationships and psychopathology in developmental perspective: reflections on progress and promise. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychology. 2005;34:87-92.
-
33Wagner F, Camey SA, Trentini CM. Análise fatorial confirmatória da escala de inteligência Wechsler abreviada: versão português brasileiro. Aval Psicol. 2014;13:383-9.
-
34Fonseca RP, Parente MA, Cote H, Ska B, Joanette Y. Bateria MAC - Bateria Montreal de avaliação da comunicação. São Paulo: Pro-Fono; 2008.
-
35Faísca L, Afonseca S, Brüne M, Gonçalves G, Gomes A, Martins AT. Portuguese adaptation of a Faux Pas Test and a theory of mind picture stories task. Psychopathology. 2016;15:17.
-
36George D, Mallery P. SPSS for Windows step by step: a simple guide and reference. 11.0 update. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2002.
-
37Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15:155-63.
-
38Polit DF. Getting serious about test–retest reliability: a critique of retest research and some recommendations. Qual Life Res. 2014;23:1713-20.
-
39Gresham FM. Assessment of children’s social skills. J School Psychology. 1981;19:120-34.
-
40John K, Gammon GD, Prusoff BA, Warner V. The Social Adjustment Inventory for Children and Adolescents (SAICA): testing for a new semistructured interview. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26:898-911.
-
41Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social skills improvement system: rating scales. Bloomington: Pearson Assessments; 2008.
-
42Fernández-Jaén A, Fernández-Mayoralas DM, López-Arribas S, Pardos-Véglia A, Muñiz-Borrega B, García-Savaté C, et al. Social and leadership abilities in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: relation with cognitive-attentional capacities. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2012;40:136-46.
-
43Aduen PA, Day TN, Kofler MJ, Harmon SL, Wells EL, Sarver DE. Social problems in ADHD: is it a skills acquisition or performance problem? J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2018;40:440-51.
-
44Fernandez-Sotos P, Navarro E, Torio I, Dompablo M, Fernandez-Caballero A, Rodriguez-Jimenez R. Pharmacological interventions in social cognition deficits: a systematic mapping review. Psychiatry Res. 2018;270:57-67.
-
45Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Harvey PD. Social cognition psychometric evaluation: results of the initial psychometric study. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:494-504.
-
46Lo W, Li X, Hoskinson K, McNally K, Chung M, Lee J, Wang J, et al. Pediatric stroke impairs Theory of Mind performance. J Child Neurol. 2020;35:228-34.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
06 May 2022 -
Date of issue
2022
History
-
Received
27 Sept 2020 -
Accepted
19 Feb 2021